#Edit this page Wikipedia (en) copyright Wikipedia Atom feed Placebo From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Jump to: navigation, search For other uses, see Placebo (disambiguation) and Placebo effect (disambiguation). "Sugar pill" redirects here. For Kids of 88 album, see Sugarpills. The placebo effect can be produced by inert tablets, by sham surgery, and by false information, such as when electrical stimulation is turned "off" in those with Parkinson's disease implanted brain electrodes.^[1] A placebo (/pləˈsiboʊ/ plə-SEE-boh; Latin placēbō, "I shall please"^[2] from placeō, "I please") is a simulated or otherwise medically ineffectual treatment for a disease or other medical condition intended to deceive the recipient. Sometimes patients given a placebo treatment will have a perceived or actual improvement in a medical condition, a phenomenon commonly called the placebo effect. In medical research, placebos are given as control treatments and depend on the use of measured deception. Common placebos include inert tablets, vehicle infusions, sham surgery,^[3] and other procedures based on false information.^[1] However, placebos can also have a surprisingly positive effect on a patient who knows that the given treatment is without any active drug, as compared with a control group who knowingly did not get a placebo.^[4] In one common placebo procedure, however, a patient is given an inert pill, told that it may improve his/her condition, but not told that it is in fact inert. Such an intervention may cause the patient to believe the treatment will change his/her condition; and this belief may produce a subjective perception of a therapeutic effect, causing the patient to feel their condition has improved — or an actual improvement in their condition. This phenomenon is known as the placebo effect. Placebos are widely used in medical research and medicine,^[5] and the placebo effect is a pervasive phenomenon;^[5] in fact, it is part of the response to any active medical intervention.^[6] Archie Cochrane suggested in 1972^[7] "It is important to distinguish the very respectable, conscious use of placebos. The effect of placebos has been shown by randomized controlled trials to be very large. Their use in the correct place is to be encouraged […]" The placebo effect points to the importance of perception and the brain's role in physical health. However, the use of placebos as treatment in clinical medicine (as opposed to laboratory research) is ethically problematic as it introduces deception and dishonesty into the doctor-patient relationship.^[8] The United Kingdom Parliamentary Committee on Science and Technology has stated that: "...prescribing placebos... usually relies on some degree of patient deception" and "prescribing pure placebos is bad medicine. Their effect is unreliable and unpredictable and cannot form the sole basis of any treatment on the NHS."^[3] Since the publication of Henry K. Beecher's The Powerful Placebo ^[9] in 1955, the phenomenon has been considered to have clinically important effects.^[10] This view was notably challenged when, in 2001, a systematic review of clinical trials concluded that there was no evidence of clinically important effects, except perhaps in the treatment of pain and continuous subjective outcomes.^[10] The article received a flurry of criticism,^[11] but the authors later published a Cochrane review with similar conclusions (updated as of 2010^[update]).^[12] Most studies have attributed the difference from baseline until the end of the trial to a placebo effect, but the reviewers examined studies which had both placebo and untreated groups in order to distinguish the placebo effect from the natural progression of the disease.^[10] However these conclusions have been criticized because of the great variety of diseases—more than 40—in this metastudy. The effect of placebo is very different in different diseases. By pooling quite different diseases the results can be leveled out. Contents * 1 Definitions, effects, and ethics * 2 History * 3 Mechanism of the effect + 3.1 Expectancy and conditioning + 3.2 Placebo effect and the brain + 3.3 Brain and body + 3.4 Evolved health regulation * 4 Clinical utility + 4.1 Duration + 4.2 Clinical significance + 4.3 Negative effects + 4.4 Doctor-patient relationship * 5 The individual + 5.1 Who is affected + 5.2 Individual differences + 5.3 Genes * 6 Symptoms and conditions + 6.1 Pain + 6.2 Depression + 6.3 Gastric and duodenal ulcers + 6.4 Chronic fatigue syndrome + 6.5 List of medical conditions * 7 Effects on research + 7.1 Placebo-controlled studies + 7.2 Nocebo + 7.3 Placebo ingredients * 8 See also * 9 References * 10 External links Definitions, effects, and ethics[edit] See also: Medical ethics A placebo has been defined as "a substance or procedure… that is objectively without specific activity for the condition being treated".^[11] Under this definition, a wide variety of things can be placebos and exhibit a placebo effect. Pharmacological substances administered through any means can act as placebos, including pills, creams, inhalants, and injections. Medical devices such as ultrasound can act as placebos.^[13]^[14] Sham surgery,^[15]^[16]^[17] sham electrodes implanted in the brain,^[1] and sham acupuncture, either with sham needles or on fake acupuncture points, have all exhibited placebo effects.^[18] Bedding not treated to reduce allergies has been used as a placebo to control for treated bedding.^[19] The physician has even been called a placebo;^[20] a study found that patient recovery can be increased by words that suggest the patient "would be better in a few days", and if the patient is given treatment, that "the treatment would certainly make him better" rather than negative words such as "I am not sure that the treatment I am going to give you will have an effect".^[21] The placebo effect may be a component of pharmacological therapies: Pain killing and anxiety reducing drugs that are infused secretly without an individual's knowledge are less effective than when a patient knows they are receiving them. Likewise, the effects of stimulation from implanted electrodes in the brains of those with advanced Parkinson's disease are greater when they are aware they are receiving this stimulation.^[22] Sometimes administering or prescribing a placebo merges into fake medicine. The placebo effect has sometimes been defined as a physiological effect caused by the placebo, but Moerman and Jonas have pointed out that this seems illogical, as a placebo is an inert substance that does not directly cause anything. Instead they introduced the term "meaning response" for the meaning that the brain associates with the placebo, which causes a physiological placebo effect. They propose that the placebo, which may be unethical, could be avoided entirely if doctors comfort and encourage their patients' health.^[11] Ernst and Resch also attempted to distinguish between the "true" and "perceived" placebo effect, as they argued that some of the effects attributed to the placebo effect could be due to other factors.^[23] The placebo effect has been controversial throughout history. Notable medical organizations have endorsed it,^[24] but in 1903 Richard Cabot concluded that it should be avoided because it is deceptive. Newman points out the "placebo paradox", – it may be unethical to use a placebo, but also unethical "not to use something that heals". He suggests to solve this dilemma by appropriating the meaning response in medicine, that is make use of the placebo effect, as long as the "one administering… is honest, open, and believes in its potential healing power".^[8] Another possible resolution of the ethical dilemma might come from the "honest placebo" effect found in a 2010 study^[4] carried out by researchers in the Program in Placebo Studies at the Harvard Medical School, where patients with irritable bowel syndrome experienced a significant beneficial effect even though they were told the pills they were taking were placebos, as compared to a control group who received no pills. Although the placebo effect and theories on its underlying mechanisms are mostly understood in terms of human psychology, studies have also indicated that non-human animals such as dogs can also have symptoms reduced by placebo treatments.^[25] History[edit] Main article: Placebo in history The word 'placebo', Latin for "I will please", dates back to a Latin translation of the Bible by St Jerome.^[26] It was first used in a medicinal context in the 18th century. In 1785 it was defined as a "commonplace method or medicine" and in 1811 it was defined as "any medicine adapted more to please than to benefit the patient", sometimes with a derogatory implication^[27] but not with the implication of no effect.^[28] Placebos were widespread in medicine until the 20th century, and they were sometimes endorsed as necessary deceptions.^[24] In 1903 Richard Cabot said that he was brought up to use placebos,^[24] but he ultimately concluded by saying that "I have not yet found any case in which a lie does not do more harm than good".^[8] In 1961 Henry K. Beecher found^[29] that surgeons he categorized as enthusiasts relieved their patients' chest pain and heart problems more than skeptic surgeons.^[8] In 1961 Walter Kennedy introduced the word nocebo.^[24] Beginning in the 1960s, the placebo effect became widely recognized and placebo controlled trials became the norm in the approval of new medications.^[30] Later, researchers became interested in understanding the placebo effect, rather than just controlling for its effects, and in 2011, a Program in Placebo Studies was established at the Harvard Medical School. Mechanism of the effect[edit] The placebo effect is highly variable in its magnitude and reliability and is typically strongest in measures of subjective symptoms (e.g., pain) and typically weak-to-nonexistent in objective measures of health points (e.g., blood pressure, infection clearance).^[citation needed] A 2001 meta-analysis of clinical trials with placebo groups and no-treatment groups found no evidence for a placebo effect on objectively measured outcomes and possible small benefits in studies with continuous subjective outcomes (particularly pain).^[10] A 2004 follow-up analysis found similar results and increased evidence of bias in smaller trials that calls into question the apparent placebo effect on subjective outcomes.^[31] Because the placebo response is simply the patient response that cannot be attributed to an investigational intervention, there are multiple possible components of a measured placebo effect. These components having varying relevance depending on study design and the types of observations.^[32] While there is some evidence that placebo interventions can alter levels of hormones^[33] or endogenous opioids,^[34] other prominent components include expectancy effects, regression to the mean,^[35]^[36] and flawed research methodologies. Expectancy and conditioning[edit] The placebo effect is related to the perceptions and expectations of the patient; if the substance is viewed as helpful, it can heal, but, if it is viewed as harmful, it can cause negative effects, which is known as the nocebo effect. In 1985, Irving Kirsch hypothesized that placebo effects are produced by the self-fulfilling effects of response expectancies, in which the belief that one will feel different leads a person to actually feel different.^[37] According to this theory, the belief that one has received an active treatment can produce the subjective changes thought to be produced by the real treatment. Placebos can act similarly through classical conditioning, wherein a placebo and an actual stimulus are used simultaneously until the placebo is associated with the effect from the actual stimulus.^[38] Both conditioning and expectations play a role in placebo effect,^[39] and make different kinds of contribution. Conditioning has a longer-lasting effect,^[40] and can affect earlier stages of information processing.^[41] The expectancy effect can be enhanced through factors such as the enthusiasm of the doctor, differences in size and color of placebo pills, or the use of other interventions such as injections. In one study, the response to a placebo increased from 44% to 62% when the doctor treated them with "warmth, attention, and confidence."^[42] Expectancy effects have been found to occur with a range of substances. Those that think that a treatment will work display a stronger placebo effect than those that do not, as evidenced by a study of acupuncture.^[43]^[44] Because the placebo effect is based upon expectations and conditioning, the effect disappears if the patient is told that their expectations are unrealistic, or that the placebo intervention is ineffective. A conditioned pain reduction can be totally removed when its existence is explained.^[45] It has also been reported of subjects given placebos in a trial of anti-depressants, that "Once the trial was over and the patients who had been given placebos were told as much, they quickly deteriorated."^[46] A placebo described as a muscle relaxant will cause muscle relaxation and, if described as the opposite, muscle tension.^[47] A placebo presented as a stimulant will have this effect on heart rhythm, and blood pressure, but, when administered as a depressant, the opposite effect.^[48] The perceived consumption of caffeine has been reported to cause similar effects even when decaffeinated coffee is consumed,^[49]^[50] although a 2003 study found only limited support for this.^[51] Placebos represented as alcohol can cause intoxication^[52] and sensorimotor impairment.^[53] Perceived ergogenic aids can increase endurance,^[54] speed^[55] and weight-lifting ability,^[56] leading to the question of whether placebos should be allowed in sport competition.^[57] Placebos can help smokers quit.^[58] Perceived allergens that are not truly allergenic can cause allergies.^[59] Interventions such as psychotherapy can have placebo effects.^[60]^pp 164–173 The effect has been observed in the transplantation of human embryonic neurons into the brains of those with advanced Parkinson's disease.^[61] Because placebos are dependent upon perception and expectation, various factors that change the perception can increase the magnitude of the placebo response. For example, studies have found that the color and size of the placebo pill makes a difference, with "hot-colored" pills working better as stimulants while "cool-colored" pills work better as depressants. Capsules rather than tablets seem to be more effective, and size can make a difference.^[62] One researcher has found that big pills increase the effect^[63] while another has argued that the effect is dependent upon cultural background.^[64] More pills,^[65] branding,^[66] past experience,^[67] and high price^[68] increase the effect of placebo pills. Injection^[69] and acupuncture^[18] have larger effect than pills. Proper adherence to placebos is associated with decreased mortality.^[70] Motivation may contribute to the placebo effect. The active goals of an individual changes his/her somatic experience by altering the detection and interpretation of expectation-congruent symptoms, and by changing the behavioral strategies a person pursues.^[71]^[72] Motivation may link to the meaning through which people experience illness and treatment. Such meaning is derived from the culture in which they live and which informs them about the nature of illness and how it responds to treatment. Research into the placebo treatment of gastric and duodenal ulcers shows that this varies widely with society.^[11] The placebo effect in treating gastric ulcers is low in Brazil, higher in northern Europe (Denmark, Netherlands), and extremely high in Germany. However, the placebo effect in treating hypertension is lower in Germany than elsewhere.^[73] Social observation can induce a placebo effect such when a person sees another having reduced pain following what they believe is a pain reducing procedure.^[74] The placebo effect can work selectively, under the influence of various psychological factors. If a placebo cream is applied on one hand with the expectation that it is an analgesic, it will reduce pain only in that hand and not elsewhere on the body.^[75] If a person is given a placebo under one name, and they respond, they will respond in the same way on a later occasion to that placebo under that name but not if under another.^[76] Placebo effect and the brain[edit] Functional imaging upon placebo analgesia shows that it links to the activation, and increased functional correlation between this activation, in the anterior cingulate, prefrontal, orbitofrontal and insular cortices, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, the brainstem periaqueductal gray matter,^[77]^[78]^[79] and the spinal cord.^[80]^[81]^[82]^[83] These changes can act upon the brain's early stages of information processing: Research using evoked brain potentials upon painful laser pulses, for example, finds placebo effects upon the N2–P2, a biphasic negative–positive complex response, the N2 peak of which is at about 230 ms, and the P2 one at about 380 ms.^[41] They occur not only during placebo analgesia but after receiving the analgesic placebo (the areas are different here, and involve the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex and inferior parietal lobule).^[84] Different areas in the higher brain have different functions. The prefrontal involvement could be related to recalling the placebo and maintaining its cognitive presence in a "self-reinforcing feedback loop" (during pain an individual recalls having taken the placebo and reduced pain reinforces its status as an analgesic).^[85] The rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) and its subcortical connectivity could be related to the expectation of potential pain stimuli^[86]^[87] The higher brain works by regulating subcortical processes. High placebo responses link with enhanced dopamine and mu-opioid activity in the circuitry for reward responses and motivated behavior of the nucleus accumbens, and, on the converse, anti-analgesic nocebos responses were associated with deactivation in this part of the brain of dopamine and opioid release.^[78] (It has been known that placebo analgesia depends upon the release in the brain of endogenous opioids since 1978.^[88]) Such analgesic placebos activation changes processing lower down in the brain by enhancing the descending inhibition through the periaqueductal gray^[78] on spinal nociceptive reflexes, while the expectations of anti-analgesic nocebos acts in the opposite way to block this.^[80] The brain is also involved in less-studied ways upon nonanalgesic placebo effects: * Parkinson's disease: Placebo relief is associated with the release of dopamine in the brain.^[89] * Depression: Placebos reducing depression affect many of the same areas that are activated by antidepressants with the addition of the prefrontal cortex^[90]^[91] * Caffeine: Placebo-caffeinated coffee causes an increase in bilateral dopamine release in the thalamus.^[92] * Glucose: The expectation of an intravenous injection of glucose increases the release of dopamine in the basal ganglia of men (but not women).^[93] * Methylphenidate: The expectation of intravenous injection of this drug in inexperienced drug users increased the release of dopamine in the ventral cingulate gyrus and nucleus accumbens, with this effect being largest in those with no prior experience of the drug.^[94] Present functional imaging upon placebo analgesia has been summarized as showing that the placebo response is "mediated by "top-down" processes dependent on frontal cortical areas that generate and maintain cognitive expectancies. Dopaminergic reward pathways may underlie these expectancies".^[95] "Diseases lacking major 'top-down' or cortically based regulation may be less prone to placebo-related improvement".^[96] Brain and body[edit] For more details on this topic, see neural top down control of physiology. The brain has control over the body processes affected by placebos. Pain, muscle fatigue, and fever are directly organized by the brain^[citation needed]. Other processes usually regulated by the body such as the immune system are also controlled indirectly through the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system^[citation needed]. Research upon conditioning in animals shows the brain can learn control over them^[citation needed]. In conditioning, a neutral stimulus saccharin is paired in a drink with an agent that produces an unconditioned response. For example, that agent might be cyclophosphamide that causes immunosuppression. After learning this pairing, the taste of saccharin by itself through neural top-down control created immunosuppression, as a new conditioned response.^[97] Such conditioning has been found to affect a diverse variety of not just basic physiological processes in the immune system but ones such as serum iron levels, oxidative DNA damage levels, and insulin secretion. This work was originally done on rats, however the same conditioning of basic physiological processes can also occur in humans^[citation needed]. Recent reviews have argued the placebo effect is due to top-down control by the brain for immunity^[98] and pain.^[99] Pacheco-López and colleagues have raised the possibility of "neocortical-sympathetic-immune axis providing neuroanatomical substrates that might explain the link between placebo/conditioned and placebo/expectation responses."^[98]^:441 A recent fMRI study has shown that a placebo can reduce pain-related neural activity in the spinal cord, indicating that placebo effects can extend beyond the brain.^[100] Ambox current red.svg Parts of this article (those related to PMID 23880289) are outdated. Please update this article to reflect recent events or newly available information. (December 2013) Evolved health regulation[edit] Evolutionary medicine identifies many symptoms such as fever, pain, and sickness behavior as evolved responses to protect or enhance the recovery from infection and injury. Fever, for example, is an evolved self-treatment that removes bacteria or viruses through raised body temperature. These evolved responses, however, also have a cost that depending upon circumstances can outweigh their benefit (due to this, for example, there is a reduction in fever during malnutrition or late pregnancy). According to the health management system theory proposed by Nicholas Humphrey, the brain has been selected to ensure that evolved responses are deployed only when the cost benefit is biologically advantageous. To do this, the brain factors in a variety of information sources, including the likelihood derived from beliefs that the body will get well without deploying its costly evolved responses. One such source of information is the knowledge the body is receiving care and treatment. The placebo effect in this perspective arises when false information about medications misleads the health management system about the likelihood of getting well so that it selects not to deploy an evolved self-treatment.^[101] Clinical utility[edit] Duration[edit] Placebo effects can last for a long time: over 8 weeks for panic disorder,^[102] 6 months for angina pectoris,^[103] and two and half years for rheumatoid arthritis.^[104] Placebo effects after verbal suggestion for mild pain can be robust and still exist after being repeated ten times even if they have no actual pharmacological pain killing action.^[45] Clinical significance[edit] Hróbjartsson and Peter Gøtzsche published a study in 2001^[10] and a follow-up study in 2004^[31] questioning the nature of the placebo effect. The studies were performed as two meta-analyses. They found that in studies with a binary outcome, meaning patients were classified as improved or not improved, the placebo group had no statistically significant improvement over the no-treatment group. Likewise, there was no significant placebo effect in studies in which objective outcomes (such as blood pressure) were measured by an independent observer. The placebo effect could be documented only in studies in which the outcomes (improvement or failure to improve) were reported by the subjects themselves. The authors concluded that the placebo effect does not have "powerful clinical effects," (objective effects) and that patient-reported improvements (subjective effects) in pain were small and could not be clearly distinguished from reporting bias. Other researchers (Wampold et al.) re-analysed the same data from the 2001 meta-analysis and concluded that the placebo effects for objective symptom measures are comparable to placebo effects for subjective ones and that the placebo effect can exceed the effect of the active treatment by 20% for disorders amenable to the placebo effect,^[105]^[106] a conclusion which Hróbjartsson & Gøtzsche described as "powerful spin".^[107] Another group of researchers noted the dramatically different conclusions between these two sets of authors despite nearly identical meta-analytic results, and suggested that placebo effects are indeed significant but small in magnitude.^[108] Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche's conclusion has been criticised on several grounds. Their meta-analysis covered studies into a highly mixed group of conditions. It has been reported that for measurements in peripheral organs the placebo effect seems to be more effective in achieving improvements in physical parameters (such as decreasing hypertension, improving FEV1 in asthma sufferers, or decreasing prostatic hyperplasia or anal fissure) than in improving biochemical parameters (such as cholesterol or cortisol) in various conditions such as venous leg ulcers, Crohn's disease, urinary tract infection, and chronic heart failure.^[109] Placebos also do not work as strongly in clinical trials because the subjects do not know whether they might be getting a real treatment or a sham one. Where studies are made of placebos in which people think they are receiving actual treatment (rather than merely its possibility) the placebo effect has been observed.^[110] Other writers have argued that the placebo effect can be reliably demonstrated under appropriate conditions.^[111] In another update by Hróbjartsson & Gøtzsche, published as a 2010 Cochrane systematic review which confirms and modifies their previous work, over 200 trials investigating 60 clinical conditions were included. Placebo interventions were again not found to have important clinical effects in general but may influence patient-reported outcomes in some situations, especially pain and nausea, although it was "difficult to distinguish patient-reported effects of placebo from response bias". The pooled relative risk they calculated for placebo was 0.93 (effect of only 7%) but significant. Effects were also found for phobia and asthma but were uncertain due to high risk of bias. In other conditions involving three or more trials, there was no statistically significant effect for smoking, dementia, depression, obesity, hypertension, insomnia and anxiety, although confidence intervals were wide. Several clinical (physical placebos, patient-involved outcomes, falsely informing patients there was no placebo) and methodological (small sample size, explicit aim of studying the placebo effect) factors were associated with higher effects of placebo. Despite low effects in general and the risk of bias, the authors acknowledged that large effects of placebo interventions may occur in certain situations.^[112] Negative effects[edit] Similar to the placebo effect, inert substances have the potential to cause negative effects via the "nocebo effect" (Latin nocebo = "I will harm"). In this effect, giving an inert substance has negative consequences.^[113] Another negative consequence is that placebos can cause side-effects associated with real treatment.^[114] One example of this is with those that have already taken an opiate, can then show respiratory depression when given it again in the form of a placebo.^[115] Withdrawal symptoms can also occur after placebo treatment. This was found, for example, after the discontinuation of the Women's Health Initiative study of hormone replacement therapy for menopause. Women had been on placebo for an average of 5.7 years. Moderate or severe withdrawal symptoms were reported by 40.5% of those on placebo compared to 63.3% of those on hormone replacement.^[116] Doctor-patient relationship[edit] A study of Danish general practitioners found that 48% had prescribed a placebo at least 10 times in the past year.^[5] The most frequently prescribed placebos were antibiotics for viral infections, and vitamins for fatigue. Specialists and hospital-based physicians reported much lower rates of placebo use. A 2004 study in the British Medical Journal of physicians in Israel found that 60% used placebos in their medical practice, most commonly to "fend off" requests for unjustified medications or to calm a patient.^[117] The accompanying editorial concluded, "We cannot afford to dispense with any treatment that works, even if we are not certain how it does."^[118] Other researches have argued that open provision of placebos for treating ADHD in children can be effective in maintaining ADHD children on lower stimulant doses in the short term.^[119] Critics of the practice responded that it is unethical to prescribe treatments that do not work, and that telling a patient (as opposed to a research test subject) that a placebo is a real medication is deceptive and harms the doctor-patient relationship in the long run. Critics also argued that using placebos can delay the proper diagnosis and treatment of serious medical conditions.^[120] The following impracticalities exist with placebos: (See the BMJ posted responses to Spiegel's editorial rapid response online section.^[118]) * Roughly only 30% of the population seems susceptible to placebo effects, and it is not possible to determine ahead of time whether a placebo will work or not. (However the placebo effect is zero in studies of blood poisoning and up to 80% in studies of wound on the duodenum). * Patients rightfully want immediate relief or improvement from their illness or symptoms. A non-placebo can often provide that, while a placebo might not. * Legitimate doctors and pharmacists could open themselves up to charges of fraud since sugar pills would cost pennies or cents for a bottle, but the price for a "real" medication would have to be charged to avoid making the patient suspicious. About 25% of physicians in both the Danish and Israeli studies used placebos as a diagnostic tool to determine if a patient's symptoms were real, or if the patient was malingering. Both the critics and defenders of the medical use of placebos agreed that this was unethical. The British Medical Journal editorial said, "That a patient gets pain relief from a placebo does not imply that the pain is not real or organic in origin...the use of the placebo for 'diagnosis' of whether or not pain is real is misguided." The placebo administration may prove to be a useful treatment in some specific cases where recommended drugs cannot be used. For example, burn patients who are experiencing respiratory problems cannot often be prescribed opioid (morphine) or opioid derivatives (pethidine), as these can cause further respiratory depression. In such cases placebo injections (normal saline, etc.) are of use in providing real pain relief to burn patients if those not in delirium are told they are being given a powerful dose of painkiller. Referring specifically to homeopathy, the House of Commons of the United Kingdom Science and Technology Committee has stated: In the Committee's view, homeopathy is a placebo treatment and the Government should have a policy on prescribing placebos. The Government is reluctant to address the appropriateness and ethics of prescribing placebos to patients, which usually relies on some degree of patient deception. Prescribing of placebos is not consistent with informed patient choice-which the Government claims is very important-as it means patients do not have all the information needed to make choice meaningful. Beyond ethical issues and the integrity of the doctor-patient relationship, prescribing pure placebos is bad medicine. Their effect is unreliable and unpredictable and cannot form the sole basis of any treatment on the NHS.^[3] A survey in the United States of more than 10,000 physicians came to the result that while 24% of physicians would prescribe a treatment that is a placebo simply because the patient wanted treatment, 58% would not, and for the remaining 18%, it would depend on the circumstances.^[121] The individual[edit] Who is affected[edit] Placebos do not work for everyone.^[122]^[123] Henry K. Beecher, in a paper in 1955,^[9] suggested placebo effects occurred in about 35% of people. However, the response rate is wide, ranging from 0% up to nearly everyone. In a dental postoperative pain model, placebo analgesia occurred in 39%.^[123] In research upon ischemic arm pain, placebo analgesia was found in 27%.^[122] The placebo analgesia rate for cutaneous healing of left hand skin was 56%.^[124] Though not everyone responds to a placebo, neither does everyone respond to an active drug. The percentage of patients who reported relief following placebo (39%) is similar to the percentage following 4 mg (36%) and 6 mg (50%) of hidden morphine.^[125] Individual differences[edit] In the 1950s, there was considerable research to find whether there was a specific personality to those that responded to placebos. The findings could not be replicated^[126] and it is now thought to have no effect.^[127] The desire for relief from pain, "goal motivation", and how far pain is expected to be relieved increases placebo analgesia.^[71] Another factor increasing the effectiveness of placebos is the degree to which a person attends to their symptoms, "somatic focus".^[72] Individual variation in response to analgesic placebos has been linked to regional neurochemical differences in the internal affective state of the individuals experiencing pain.^[128] Those with Alzheimer's disease lose the capacity to be influenced by placebos, and this is attributed to the loss of their prefrontal cortex dependent capacity to have expectations.^[129] Children seem to have greater response than adults to placebos.^[130] Genes[edit] In social anxiety disorder (SAD) an inherited variant of the gene for tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (enzyme that synthesizes the neurotransmitter serotonin) is linked to reduced amygdala activity and greater susceptibility to the placebo effect.^[131]^[132]^[133] The authors note "additional work is necessary to elucidate the generalizability of the findings". In a 2012 study, variations on the COMT (catechol-O-methyltransferase) gene related to dopamine release are found to be critical in the placebo effect among the patients with irritable bowel syndrome participating in the trial, a research group in Harvard Medical School reported. Patients with a variation of met/met, for having two copies of the methionine allele were shown to be more likely to respond to the placebo treatment, while the variation of val/val, for their two copies of valine allele responded the least. The response of patients with one copy each of methionine and valine fell in the middle. Release of dopamine in patients with the met/met variations is thought to link to reward and 'confirmation bias' which enhance the sense that the treatment is working. The role of the COMT gene variations are expected to be more prominent in studies where patients report more subjective conditions such as pain and fatigue rather than objective physiological measurements.^[134]^[135] Symptoms and conditions[edit] The placebo effect occurs more strongly in some conditions than others. Dylan Evans has suggested that placebos work most strongly upon conditions such as pain, swelling, stomach ulcers, depression, and anxiety that have been linked with activation of the acute-phase response.^[60] Pain[edit] The placebo effect is believed to reduce pain in two different ways. One way is by the placebo initiating the release of endorphins, which are natural pain killers produced by the brain.^[136] The other way is the placebo changing the patient's perception of pain. "A person might reinterpret a sharp pain as uncomfortable tingling."^[137] Placebo analgesia is more likely to work the more severe the pain.^[138] One study found that for postoperative pain following the extraction of the third molar, saline injected while telling the patient it was a powerful painkiller was as potent as a 6–8 mg dose of morphine.^[125] Most research reports average reduction for a group of people, but this can be lower (some people do not respond). In one study using injection of capsaicin below the skin found that this reduced group average pain compared to no placebo by ~46% to ~57%.^[75] Another measure is the ability to endure pain. In one study, placebos increased this on average by about 3.5 minutes compared to just under 14 minutes without it.^[139] The average strength of placebos upon pain on a visual analog scale is 2 out of 10 units.^[127]^[140] Individuals who respond to placebos may show even greater effects, up to 5 out of 10 units.^[122] Depression[edit] In 1998, a meta-analysis of published antidepressant trials found that 75% of the effectiveness of anti-depressant medication is due to the placebo effect and other non-specific effects, rather than the treatment itself.^[141] Later, meta-analyses including data from unpublished trials found that the overall difference between drug and placebo is not clinically significant except in cases of very extreme depression,^[142]^[143] Another meta-analysis found that 79% of depressed patients receiving placebo remained well (for 12 weeks after an initial 6–8 weeks of successful therapy) compared to 93% of those receiving antidepressants.^[144] A meta-analysis in 2002 found a 30% reduction in suicide and attempted suicide in the placebo groups compared to a 40% reduction in the treated groups.^[145] A 2002 article in The Washington Post titled "Against Depression, a Sugar Pill Is Hard to Beat" summarized research as follows: In the majority of trials conducted by drug companies in recent decades, sugar pills have done as well as -- or better than -- antidepressants. Companies have had to conduct numerous trials to get two that show a positive result, which is the Food and Drug Administration's minimum for approval. The makers of Prozac had to run five trials to obtain two that were positive, and the makers of Paxil and Zoloft had to run even more.^[46] Gastric and duodenal ulcers[edit] A meta-study of 31 placebo-controlled trials of the gastric acid secretion inhibitor drug cimetidine in the treatment of gastric or duodenal ulcers found that placebo treatments, in many cases, were as effective as active drugs: of the 1692 patients treated in the 31 trials, 76% of the 916 treated with the drug were "healed", and 48% of the 776 treated with placebo were "healed".^[11]^[146] These results were confirmed by the direct post-treatment endoscopy. It was also found that German placebos were "stronger" than others; and that, overall, different physicians evoked quite different placebo responses in the same clinical trial.^[146]^:15 Moreover, in many of these trials the gap between the active drugs and the placebo controls was "not because [the trials' constituents] had high drug effectiveness, but because they had low placebo effectiveness".^[146]^:13 In some trials, placebos were effective in 90% of the cases, whereas in others the placebos were effective in only 10% of the cases. It was argued that "what is demonstrated in [these] studies is not enhanced healing in drug groups but reduced healing in placebo groups".^[146]^:14 It was also noted the results of two studies (one conducted in Germany, the other in Denmark), which examined "ulcer relapse in healed patients" showed that the rate of relapse amongst those "healed" by the active drug treatment was five times that of those "healed" by the placebo treatment.^[146]^:14–15 Chronic fatigue syndrome[edit] It was previously assumed that placebo response rates in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) are unusually high, "at least 30% to 50%", because of the subjective reporting of symptoms and the fluctuating nature of the condition. According to a meta-analysis and contrary to conventional wisdom, the pooled response rate in the placebo group was 19.6%, even lower than in some other medical conditions. The authors offer possible explanations for this result: CFS is widely understood to be difficult to treat, which could reduce expectations of improvement. In context of evidence showing placebos do not have powerful clinical effects when compared to no treatment, a low rate of spontaneous remission in CFS could contribute to reduced improvement rates in the placebo group. Intervention type also contributed to the heterogeneity of the response. Low patient and provider expectations regarding psychological treatment may explain particularly low placebo responses to psychiatric treatments.^[147] List of medical conditions[edit] The effect of placebo treatments (an inert pill unless otherwise noted) has been studied for the following medical conditions. Many of these citations concern research showing that active treatments are effective, but that placebo effects exist as well. * ADHD: adult,^[148] child^[119] * Amalgam fillings: attributed symptoms (inert "chelation" therapy)^[149] * Anxiety disorders^[150]^[151] * Asthma (water aerosol inhalant)^[152] * Asthma^[153]^[154] * Autism: language and behavior problems^[155]^[156] * Benign prostatic enlargement^[157] * Binge eating disorder^[158] * Bipolar mania^[159] * Cough^[6] * Crohn's disease^[160] * Depression (light treatment; low red light placebo)^[161] * Depression^[141]^[162]^[163]^[164] * Dyspepsia and Stomach motility^[165] * Epilepsy^[166] * Erectile dysfunction^[167] * Food allergy: ability to eat ill-making foods^[73] ^p. 54 * Gastric and duodenal ulcers^[73]^[146]^[168] * Headache^[169] * Heart failure, congestive^[170] * Herpes simplex^[171] * Hypertension: mild and moderate^[69]^[172] * Irritable bowel syndrome^[173]^[174] * Migraine prophylaxis^[175] * Multiple sclerosis^[176] * Nausea: gastric activity^[177] * Nausea: chemotherapy^[178] * Nausea and vomiting : postoperative (sham acupuncture)^[179] * Pain^[127]^[180] * Panic disorders^[181] * Parkinson's disease^[182]^[183] * Pathological gambling^[184] * Premenstrual dysphoric disorder.^[185] * Psoriatic arthritis^[186] * Reflux esophagitis^[187] * Restless leg syndrome^[188] * Rheumatic diseases^[189] * Sexual dysfunction: women^[190] * Social phobia^[191] * Third molar extraction swelling (sham ultra-sound)^[13]^[14] * Ulcerative colitis^[192] * Vulvar vestibulitis^[193] Effects on research[edit] Placebo-controlled studies[edit] Main article: Placebo-controlled studies The placebo effect makes it more difficult to evaluate new treatments. The placebo effect in such clinical trials is weaker than in normal therapy since the subjects are not sure whether the treatment they are receiving is active.^[110] Apparent benefits of a new treatment (usually a drug but not necessarily so) may not derive from the treatment but from the placebo effect. This is particularly likely, given that new therapies seem to have greater placebo effects.^[194] Clinical trials control for this effect by including a group of subjects that receives a sham treatment. The subjects in such trials are blinded as to whether they receive the treatment or a placebo. If a person is given a placebo under one name, and they respond, they will respond in the same way on a later occasion to that placebo under that name but not if under another.^[195] Clinical trials are often double-blinded so that the researchers also do not know which test subjects are receiving the active or placebo treatment.The placebo effect in such clinical trials is weaker than in normal therapy since the subjects are not sure whether the treatment they are receiving is active.^[110] Knowingly giving a person a placebo when there is an effective treatment available is a bioethically complex issue. While placebo-controlled trials might provide information about the effectiveness of a treatment, it denies some patients what could be the best available (if unproven) treatment. Informed consent is usually required for a study to be considered ethical, including the disclosure that some test subjects will receive placebo treatments. The ethics of placebo-controlled studies have been debated in the revision process of the Declaration of Helsinki.^[196] Of particular concern has been the difference between trials comparing inert placebos with experimental treatments, versus comparing the best available treatment with an experimental treatment; and differences between trials in the sponsor's developed countries versus the trial's targeted developing countries.^[197] Nocebo[edit] Main article: Nocebo In the opposite effect, a patient who disbelieves in a treatment may experience a worsening of symptoms. This effect, now called by analogy nocebo (Latin nocebo = "I shall harm") can be measured in the same way as the placebo effect, e.g., when members of a control group receiving an inert substance report a worsening of symptoms. The recipients of the inert substance may nullify the placebo effect intended by simply having a negative attitude towards the effectiveness of the substance prescribed, which often leads to a nocebo effect, which is not caused by the substance, but due to other factors, such as the patient's mentality towards his or her ability to get well, or even purely coincidental worsening of symptoms.^[113] Placebo ingredients[edit] Placebos used in clinical trials have sometimes had unintended consequences. A report in the Annals of Internal Medicine that looked at details from 150 clinical trials found that certain placebos used in the trials affected the results. For example, one study on cholesterol-lowering drugs used olive oil and corn oil in the placebo pills. However, according to the report, this "may lead to an understatement of drug benefit: The monounsaturated and polyunsaturated fatty acids of these 'placebos,' and their antioxidant and anti-inflammatory effects, can reduce lipid levels and heart disease." Another example researchers reported in the study was a clinical trial of a new therapy for cancer patients suffering from anorexia. The placebo that was used included lactose. However, since cancer patients typically face a higher risk of lactose intolerance, the placebo pill might actually have caused unintended side-effects that made the experimental drug look better in comparison.^[198] See also[edit] * Self-fulfilling prophecy References[edit] 1. ^ ^a ^b ^c Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Torre E, Bergamasco B, Colloca L, Benedetti F (November 2005). "Expectation enhances autonomic responses to stimulation of the human subthalamic limbic region". Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 19 (6): 500–9. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2005.06.004. PMID 16055306. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 2. ^ Gensini GF, Conti AA, Conti A (April 2005). "Past and present of what will please the lord: an updated history of the concept of placebo". Minerva Med 96 (2): 121–4. PMID 16172581. 3. ^ ^a ^b ^c UK Parliamentary Committee Science and Technology Committee. "Evidence Check 2: Homeopathy". 4. ^ ^a ^b Kaptchuk TJ, Friedlander E, Kelley JM, et al. (2010). "Placebos without Deception: A Randomized Controlled Trial in Irritable Bowel Syndrome". In Boutron, Isabelle. PLoS ONE 5 (12): e15591. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...515591K. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0015591. PMC 3008733. PMID 21203519. Lay summary. 5. ^ ^a ^b ^c Hróbjartsson A, Norup M (June 2003). "The use of placebo interventions in medical practice--a national questionnaire survey of Danish clinicians". Evaluation & the Health Professions 26 (2): 153–65. doi:10.1177/0163278703026002002. PMID 12789709. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 6. ^ ^a ^b Eccles R (2002). "The powerful placebo in cough studies?". Pulm Pharmacol Ther 15 (3): 251–2. doi:10.1006/pupt.2002.0364. PMID 12099783. 7. ^ Cochrane, A.L.: Effectiveness and Efficiency: Random Reflections on Health Services. The Nuffield Provincial Hospitals Trust 1972, p31. 8. ^ ^a ^b ^c ^d David H. Newman (2008). Hippocrates' Shadow. Scribner. pp. 134–59. ISBN 1-4165-5153-0. 9. ^ ^a ^b Beecher, H. K. (1955). "The powerful placebo". Journal of the American Medical Association 159 (17): 1602–1606. doi:10.1001/jama.1955.02960340022006. PMID 13271123. edit 10. ^ ^a ^b ^c ^d ^e Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2001). "Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment". New England Journal of Medicine 344 (21): 1594–1602. doi:10.1056/NEJM200105243442106. PMID 11372012. 11. ^ ^a ^b ^c ^d ^e Moerman DE, Jonas WB (2002). "Deconstructing the placebo effect and finding the meaning response". Ann Intern Med. 136 (6): 471–6. PMID 11900500. 12. ^ Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (20 January 2010). "Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions". In Hróbjartsson, Asbjørn. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 106 (1): CD003974. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003974.pub3. PMID 20091554. 13. ^ ^a ^b Ho KH, Hashish I, Salmon P, Freeman R, Harvey W (1988). "Reduction of post-operative swelling by a placebo effect". Journal of Psychosomatic Research 32 (2): 197–205. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(88)90055-4. PMID 3404502. 14. ^ ^a ^b Hashish I, Harvey W, Harris M (February 1986). "Anti-inflammatory effects of ultrasound therapy: evidence for a major placebo effect". British Journal of Rheumatology 25 (1): 77–81. doi:10.1093/rheumatology/25.1.77. PMID 2417648. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 15. ^ Cobb LA, Thomas GI, Dillard DH, Merendino KA, Bruce RA (May 1959). "An evaluation of internal-mammary-artery ligation by a double-blind technic". The New England Journal of Medicine 260 (22): 1115–8. doi:10.1056/NEJM195905282602204. PMID 13657350. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 16. ^ Benson H, McCallie DP (June 1979). "Angina pectoris and the placebo effect". The New England Journal of Medicine 300 (25): 1424–9. doi:10.1056/NEJM197906213002508. PMID 35750. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 17. ^ Moseley JB, O'Malley K, Petersen NJ, et al. (July 2002). "A controlled trial of arthroscopic surgery for osteoarthritis of the knee". The New England Journal of Medicine 347 (2): 81–8. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa013259. PMID 12110735. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 18. ^ ^a ^b Kaptchuk TJ, Stason WB, Davis RB, et al. (February 2006). "Sham device v inert pill: randomised controlled trial of two placebo treatments". BMJ 332 (7538): 391–7. doi:10.1136/bmj.38726.603310.55. PMC 1370970. PMID 16452103. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 19. ^ Holm L, Bengtsson A, van Hage-Hamsten M, Ohman S, Scheynius A (February 2001). "Effectiveness of occlusive bedding in the treatment of atopic dermatitis—a placebo-controlled trial of 12 months' duration". Allergy 56 (2): 152–8. doi:10.1034/j.1398-9995.2001.056002152.x. PMID 11167376. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 20. ^ Margo CE (1999). "The placebo effect". Surv Ophthalmol. 44 (1): 33–4. doi:10.1016/S0039-6257(99)00060-0. PMID 10466586. 21. ^ Thomas KB (1987). "General practice consultations: is there any point in being positive?". Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 294 (6581): 1200–2. doi:10.1136/bmj.294.6581.1200. PMC 1246362. PMID 3109581. 22. ^ Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F (2004). "Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson's disease". Lancet Neurol. 3 (11): 679–84. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00908-1. PMID 15488461. 23. ^ Ernst E, Resch KL (August 1995). "Concept of true and perceived placebo effects". BMJ 311 (7004): 551–3. doi:10.1136/bmj.311.7004.551. PMC 2550609. PMID 7663213. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 24. ^ ^a ^b ^c ^d de Craen AJ, Kaptchuk TJ, Tijssen JG, Kleijnen J (October 1999). "Placebos and placebo effects in medicine: historical overview". J R Soc Med 92 (10): 511–5. PMC 1297390. PMID 10692902. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 25. ^ Muã±Ana, K. R.; Zhang, D.; Patterson, E. E. (2010). "Placebo Effect in Canine Epilepsy Trials". Journal of Veterinary Internal Medicine 24 (1): 166–170. doi:10.1111/j.1939-1676.2009.0407.x. PMID 19912522. edit 26. ^ Jacobs B. (2000). "Biblical origins of placebo". J R Soc Med. 93 (4): 213–4. PMC 1297986. PMID 10844895. 27. ^ Shapiro AK (1968). "Semantics of the placebo". Psychiatr Q. 42 (4): 653–95. doi:10.1007/BF01564309. PMID 4891851. 28. ^ Kaptchuk TJ (1998). "Powerful placebo: the dark side of the randomised controlled trial". Lancet 351 (9117): 1722–5. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(97)10111-8. PMID 9734904. 29. ^ Beecher HK (July 1961). "Surgery as placebo. A quantitative study of bias". J Am Med Assoc 176 (13): 1102–7. doi:10.1001/jama.1961.63040260007008. PMID 13688614. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 30. ^ Kaptchuk, T. J. (1998). Intentional Ignorance: A History of Blind Assessment and Placebo Controls in Medicine. Bulletin of the History of Medicine, 72(3), 389-433. 31. ^ ^a ^b Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2004). "Is the placebo powerless? Update of a systematic review with 52 new randomized trials comparing placebo with no treatment". J. Intern. Med. 256 (2): 91–100. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01355.x. PMID 15257721. 32. ^ Kienle GS, Kiene H (December 1997). "The powerful placebo effect: fact or fiction?". J Clin Epidemiol 50 (12): 1311–8. PMID 9449934. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 33. ^ Kokkotou E et al. Serum correlates of the placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome. Neurogastroent Motilty 2010; 22:285-e81.http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2852478/?too l=pubmed 34. ^ Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK (November 2005). "Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect". J. Neurosci. 25 (45): 10390–402. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3458-05.2005. PMID 16280578. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 35. ^ McDonald CJ, Mazzuca SA, McCabe GP (1983). "How much of the placebo 'effect' is really statistical regression?". Stat Med 2 (4): 417–27. PMID 6369471. 36. ^ Barnett AG, van der Pols JC, Dobson AJ (February 2005). "Regression to the mean: what it is and how to deal with it". Int J Epidemiol 34 (1): 215–20. doi:10.1093/ije/dyh299. PMID 15333621. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 37. ^ Kirsch I (1985). "Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior". American Psychologist 40 (11): 1189–1202. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.40.11.1189. 38. ^ Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G (1989). "Conditioned response models of placebo phenomena: further support". Pain 38 (1): 109–16. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(89)90080-8. PMID 2780058. 39. ^ Stewart-Williams S, Podd J (2004). "The placebo effect: dissolving the expectancy versus conditioning debate". Psychol Bull 130 (2): 324–40. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.324. PMID 14979775. 40. ^ Klinger R, Soost S, Flor H, Worm M (2007). "Classical conditioning and expectancy in placebo hypoalgesia: a randomized controlled study in patients with atopic dermatitis and persons with healthy skin". Pain. 128:31-9 (1–2): 31–9. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.025. PMID 17030095. 41. ^ ^a ^b Colloca L, Tinazzi M, Recchia S, Le Pera D, Fiaschi A, Benedetti F, Valeriani M (2008). "Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses". Pain 139 (2): 306–14. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.021. PMID 18538928. 42. ^ Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, Davis RB, Kerr CE, Jacobson EE, Kirsch I, Schyner RN, Nam BH, Nguyen LT, Park M, Rivers AL, McManus C, Kokkotou E, Drossman DA, Goldman P, Lembo AJ (2008). "Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome". BMJ 336 (7651): 999–1003. doi:10.1136/bmj.39524.439618.25. PMC 2364862. PMID 18390493. 43. ^ Linde K, Witt CM, Streng A, Weidenhammer W, Wagenpfeil S, Brinkhaus B, Willich SN, Melchart D (2007). "The effect of patient expectations on outcomes in four randomized controlled trials of acupuncture in patients with chronic pain". Pain 128 (3): 264–71. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.006. PMID 17257756. 44. ^ Bausell RB, Lao L, Bergman S, Lee WL, Berman BM (2005). "Is acupuncture analgesia an expectancy effect? Preliminary evidence based on participants' perceived assignments in two placebo-controlled trials". Eval Health Prof. 28 (1): 9–26. doi:10.1177/0163278704273081. PMID 15677384. 45. ^ ^a ^b Montgomery GH, Kirsch I (1997). "Classical conditioning and the placebo effect". Pain 72 (1–2): 107–13. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00016-X. PMID 9272794. 46. ^ ^a ^b "Against Depression, a Sugar Pill Is Hard to Beat". The Washington Post. May 7, 2002. 47. ^ Flaten MA, Simonsen T, Olsen H (1999). "Drug-related information generates placebo and nocebo responses that modify the drug response". Psychosom Med. 61 (2): 250–5. PMID 10204979. 48. ^ Kirsch I (1997). "Specifying non-specifics: Psychological mechanism of the placebo effect". In Harrington A. The Placebo Effect: An Interdisciplinary Exploration. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. pp. 166–86. ISBN 978-0-674-66986-4. 49. ^ Kirsch, I., & Weixel, L. J. (1988). Double-blind versus deceptive administration of a placebo. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102(2), 319-323. 50. ^ Kirsch I., Rosadino M. J. (1993). "Do Double-Blind Studies with Informed Consent Yield Externally Valid Results - an Empirical-Test". Psychopharmacology 110 (4): 437–442. doi:10.1007/BF02244650. PMID 7870914. 51. ^ Flaten MA, Aasli O, Blumenthal TD (2003). "Expectations and placebo responses to caffeine-associated stimuli". Psychopharmacology 169 (2): 198–204. doi:10.1007/s00213-003-1497-8. PMID 12759808. 52. ^ O'Boyle DJ, Binns AS, Sumner JJ (1994). "On the efficacy of alcohol placebos in inducing feelings of intoxication". Psychopharmacology (Berl) 115 (1–2): 229–36. doi:10.1007/BF02244776. PMID 7862899. 53. ^ Fillmore MT, Mulvihill LE, Vogel-Sprott M (1994). "The expected drug and its expected effect interact to determine placebo responses to alcohol and caffeine". Psychopharmacology 115 (3): 383–8. doi:10.1007/BF02245081. PMID 7871080. 54. ^ Beedie CJ, Coleman DA, Foad AJ (2007). "Positive and negative placebo effects resulting from the deceptive administration of an ergogenic aid". Int. J. Sport Nutr. Exerc. Metab. 17: 259–269. 55. ^ Clark K, Milliken R (21 August 2000). "Today, it's "May the best swimsuit win"". US News and World Report. p. 55. , cited in: Moerman DE, Jonas WB (19 March 2002). "Deconstructing the placebo effect and finding the meaning response". Ann Intern Med 136 (6): 471–6. PMID 11900500. 56. ^ Pollo A, Carlino E, Benedetti F (July 2008). "The top-down influence of ergogenic placebos on muscle work and fatigue". Eur. J. Neurosci. 28 (2): 379–88. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06344.x. PMID 18702709. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 57. ^ Benedetti F, Pollo A, Colloca L (2007). "Opioid-mediated placebo responses boost pain endurance and physical performance – is it doping in sport competitions". J. Neurosci. 27 (44): 11934–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3330-07.2007. PMID 17978033. 58. ^ Dar R, Stronguin F, Etter JF (2005). "Assigned versus perceived placebo effects in nicotine replacement therapy for smoking reduction in Swiss smokers". J Consult Clin Psychol 73 (2): 350–3. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.350. PMID 15796644. 59. ^ Ikemi Y, Nakagawa S (1962). "A psychosomatic study of contagious dermatitis". Kyoshu Journal of Medical Science 13: 335–50. 60. ^ ^a ^b Evans, Dylan (2003). Placebo: the belief effect. London: HarperCollins. ISBN 0-00-712612-3. 61. ^ McRae C, Cherin E, Yamazaki TG, Diem G, Vo AH, Russell D, Ellgring JH, Fahn S, Greene P, Dillon S, Winfield H, Bjugstad KB, Freed CR (2004). "Effects of perceived treatment on quality of life and medical outcomes in a double-blind placebo surgery trial". Arch Gen Psychiatry 61 (4): 412–20. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.412. PMID 15066900. 62. ^ de Craen AJ, Roos PJ, Leonard de Vries A, Kleijnen J (1996). "Effect of colour of drugs: systematic review of perceived effect of drugs and of their effectiveness". BMJ 313 (7072): 1624–6. doi:10.1136/bmj.313.7072.1624. PMC 2359128. PMID 8991013. 63. ^ Buckalew LW, Ross S (1981). "Relationship of perceptual characteristics to efficacy of placebos". Psychol. Rep. 49 (3): 955–61. doi:10.2466/pr0.1981.49.3.955. PMID 7330154. 64. ^ Dolinska B (1999). "Empirical investigation into placebo effectiveness". Ir J Psych Med 16 (2): 57–58. 65. ^ Blackwell B, Bloomfield SS, Buncher CR (1972). "Demonstration to medical students of placebo responses and non-drug factors". Lancet 1 (7763): 1279–82. PMID 4113531. 66. ^ Branthwaite A, Cooper P (1981). "Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches". Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 282 (6276): 1576–8. doi:10.1136/bmj.282.6276.1576. PMC 1505530. PMID 6786566. 67. ^ Colloca L, Benedetti F (2006). "How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia". Pain 124 (1–2): 126–33. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.005. PMID 16701952. 68. ^ Waber RL, Shiv B, Carmon Z, Ariely D (2008). "Commercial features of placebo and therapeutic efficacy". JAMA 299 (9): 1016–7. doi:10.1001/jama.299.9.1016. PMID 18319411. 69. ^ ^a ^b Grenfell RF, Briggs AH, Holland WC (1961). "A double-blind study of the treatment of hypertension". JAMA 176 (2): 124–8. doi:10.1001/jama.1961.03040150040010. PMID 13708477. 70. ^ Simpson SH, Eurich DT, Majumdar SR, Padwal RS, Tsuyuki RT, Varney J, Johnson JA (2006). "A meta-analysis of the association between adherence to drug therapy and mortality". BMJ 333 (7557): 15. doi:10.1136/bmj.38875.675486.55. PMC 1488752. PMID 16790458. 71. ^ ^a ^b Geers AL, Weiland PE, Kosbab K, Landry SJ, Helfer SG (2005). "Goal activation, expectations, and the placebo effect". J Pers Soc Psychol 89 (2): 143–59. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.143. PMID 16162050. 72. ^ ^a ^b Geers AL, Helfer SG, Weiland PE, Kosbab K (2006). "Expectations and placebo response: a laboratory investigation into the role of somatic focus". J Behav Med 29 (2): 171–8. doi:10.1007/s10865-005-9040-5. PMID 16374671. 73. ^ ^a ^b ^c Moerman DE (2000). "Cultural variations in the placebo effect: ulcers, anxiety, and blood pressure". Med Anthropol Q 14 (51–72): 51–72. doi:10.1525/maq.2000.14.1.51. PMID 10812563. 74. ^ Colloca L, Benedetti F (2009). "Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning". Pain 144 (1–2): 28–34. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033. PMID 19278785. 75. ^ ^a ^b Benedetti F, Arduino C, Amanzio M (1999). "Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by target-directed expectations of analgesia". J Neurosci 19 (9): 3639–48. PMID 10212322. 76. ^ Whalley B, Hyland ME, Kirsch I (2008). "Consistency of the placebo effect". J Psychosom Res 64 (5): 537–41. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.11.007. PMID 18440407. 77. ^ Oken BS (2008). "Placebo effects: clinical aspects and neurobiology". Brain 131 (Pt 11): 2812–23. doi:10.1093/brain/awn116. PMC 2725026. PMID 18567924. 78. ^ ^a ^b ^c Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK (2008). "Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses". Arch Gen Psychiatry 65 (2): 220–31. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34. PMID 18250260. 79. ^ Lidstone SC, Stoessl AJ (2007). "Understanding the placebo effect: contributions from neuroimaging". Mol Imaging Biol 9 (4): 176–85. doi:10.1007/s11307-007-0086-3. PMID 17334853. 80. ^ ^a ^b Goffaux P, Redmond WJ, Rainville P, Marchand S (2007). "Descending analgesia--when the spine echoes what the brain expects". Pain 130 (1–2): 137–43. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.11.011. PMID 17215080. 81. ^ Matre D, Casey KL, Knardahl S (2006). "Placebo-induced changes in spinal cord pain processing". J Neurosci 26 (2): 559–63. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4218-05.2006. PMID 16407554. 82. ^ Qiu YH, Wu XY, Xu H, Sackett D (October 2009). "Neuroimaging study of placebo analgesia in humans". Neurosci Bull 25 (5): 277–82. doi:10.1007/s12264-009-0907-2. PMID 19784082. 83. ^ Zubieta JK, Stohler CS (March 2009). "Neurobiological Mechanisms of Placebo Responses". Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156: 198–210. Bibcode:2009NYASA1156..198Z. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04424.x. PMC 3073412. PMID 19338509. 84. ^ Nemoto H, Nemoto Y, Toda H, Mikuni M, Fukuyama H (2007). "Placebo analgesia: a PET study". Exp Brain Res 179 (4): 655–64. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0821-z. PMID 17287994. 85. ^ Craggs JG, Price DD, Verne GN, Perlstein WM, Robinson MM (2007). "Functional brain interactions that serve cognitive-affective processing during pain and placebo analgesia". Neuroimage 38 (4): 720–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.057. PMC 2100389. PMID 17904390. 86. ^ Bingel U, Lorenz J, Schoell E, Weiller C, Büchel C (2006). "Mechanisms of placebo analgesia: rACC recruitment of a subcortical antinociceptive network". Pain 120 (1–2): 8–15. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.027. PMID 16364549. 87. ^ Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD (2004). "Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain". Science 303 (5661): 1162–7. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1162W. doi:10.1126/science.1093065. PMID 14976306. 88. ^ Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL (1978). "The mechanism of placebo analgesia". Lancet 2 (8091): 654–7. PMID 80579. 89. ^ de la Fuente-Fernández R, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, Schulzer M, Calne DB, Stoessl AJ (2001). "Expectation and dopamine release: mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease". Science 293 (5532): 1164–6. doi:10.1126/science.1060937. PMID 11498597. 90. ^ Mayberg HS, Silva JA, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, Mahurin RK, McGinnis S, Jerabek PA (2002). "The functional neuroanatomy of the placebo effect". Am J Psychiatry 159 (5): 728–37. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.728. PMID 11986125. 91. ^ Leuchter AF, Cook IA, Witte EA, Morgan M, Abrams M (2002). "Changes in brain function of depressed subjects during treatment with placebo". Am J Psychiatry 159 (1): 122–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.1.122. PMID 11772700. 92. ^ Kaasinen V, Aalto S, Någren K, Rinne JO (2004). "Expectation of caffeine induces dopaminergic responses in humans". Eur J Neurosci 19 (8): 2352–6. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03310.x. PMID 15090062. 93. ^ Haltia LT, Rinne JO, Helin S, Parkkola R, Någren K, Kaasinen V (2008). "Effects of intravenous placebo with glucose expectation on human basal ganglia dopaminergic function". Synapse 62 (9): 682–8. doi:10.1002/syn.20541. PMID 18566972. 94. ^ Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Wong C, Jayne M, Telang F, Swanson JM (2006). "Effects of expectation on the brain metabolic responses to methylphenidate and to its placebo in non-drug abusing subjects". Neuroimage 32 (4): 1782–92. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.192. PMID 16757181. 95. ^ Faria V, Fredrikson M, Furmark T (2008). "Imaging the placebo response: a neurofunctional review". Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 18 (7): 473–85. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.03.002. PMID 18495442. 96. ^ Diederich NJ, Goetz CG (2008). "The placebo treatments in neurosciences: New insights from clinical and neuroimaging studies". Neurology 71 (9): 677–84. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000324635.49971.3d. PMID 18725593. 97. ^ Ader R, Cohen N (1975). "Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression". Psychosom Med 37 (4): 333–40. PMID 1162023. 98. ^ ^a ^b Pacheco-López G, Engler H, Niemi MB, Schedlowski M (2006). "Expectations and associations that heal: Immunomodulatory placebo effects and its neurobiology". Brain Behav Immun 20 (430–46): 430–46. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2006.05.003. PMID 16887325. 99. ^ Colloca L, Benedetti F (2005). "Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter?". Nat Rev Neurosci 6 (7): 545–52. doi:10.1038/nrn1705. PMID 15995725. 100. ^ Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, Bingel U, Büchel1 C (2009). "Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia". Science 326 (5951): 404. Bibcode:2009Sci...326..404E. doi:10.1126/science.1180142. PMID 19833962. 101. ^ Humphrey N (2002). "Great Expectations: The Evolutionary Psychology of Faith-Healing and the Placebo Effect". The Mind Made Flesh: Essays from the Frontiers of Psychology and Evolution. Oxford University Press. pp. 255–85. ISBN 978-0-19-280227-9. 102. ^ Coryell W, Noyes R (1988). "Placebo response in panic disorder". Am J Psychiatry 145 (9): 1138–40. PMID 3046384. 103. ^ Boissel JP, Philippon AM, Gauthier E, Schbath J, Destors JM (1986). "Time course of long-term placebo therapy effects in angina pectoris". Eur Heart J 7 (12): 1030–6. PMID 3104043. 104. ^ Traut EF, Passarelli EW (1957). "Placebos in the Treatment of Rheumatoid Arthritis and other Rheumatic Conditions". Ann Rheum Dis 16 (1): 18–22. doi:10.1136/ard.16.1.18. PMC 1006924. PMID 13412002. 105. ^ Wampold BE, Minami T, Tierney SC, Baskin TW, Bhati KS (2005). "The placebo is powerful: estimating placebo effects in medicine and psychotherapy from randomized clinical trials". J Clin Psychol 61 (7): 835–54. doi:10.1002/jclp.20129. PMID 15827993. 106. ^ Wampold BE, Imel ZE, Minami T (2007). "The placebo effect: "relatively large" and "robust" enough to survive another assault". J Clin Psychol 63 (4): 401–3. doi:10.1002/jclp.20350. PMID 17279522. 107. ^ Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (2007). "Powerful spin in the conclusion of Wampold et al.'s re-analysis of placebo versus no-treatment trials despite similar results as in original review". J Clin Psychol 63 (4): 373–7. doi:10.1002/jclp.20357. PMID 17279532. 108. ^ Hunsley J, Westmacott R (2007). "Interpreting the magnitude of the placebo effect: mountain or Molehill?". J Clin Psychol 63 (4): 391–9. doi:10.1002/jclp.20352. PMID 17279525. 109. ^ Meissner K, Distel H, Mitzdorf U (2007). "Evidence for placebo effects on physical but not on biochemical outcome parameters: a review of clinical trials". BMC Med 5: 3. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-5-3. PMC 1847831. PMID 17371590. 110. ^ ^a ^b ^c Vase L, Riley JL 3rd, Price DD (2003). "A comparison of placebo effects in clinical analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia". Pain 99 (3): 714–5. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00205-1. PMID 12406519. 111. ^ Barfod TS (2005). "Placebos in medicine: Placebo use is well known, placebo effect is not". BMJ 330 (7481): 45. doi:10.1136/bmj.330.7481.45. PMC 539859. PMID 15626818. 112. ^ Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC (January 2010). "Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions". In Hróbjartsson, Asbjørn. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (1): CD003974. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003974.pub3. PMID 20091554. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 113. ^ ^a ^b "The Nocebo Effect". Priory.com. 10 February 2007. Retrieved 2009-07-08. 114. ^ Shapiro AK, Chassan J, Morris LA, Frick R (1974). "Placebo induced side effects". Journal of Operational Psychiatry 6: 43–6. 115. ^ Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Baldi S, Casadio C, Cavallo A, Mancuso M, Ruffini E, Oliaro A, Maggi G (1998). "The specific effects of prior opioid exposure on placebo analgesia and placebo respiratory depression". Pain 75 (2–3): 313–9. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00010-4. PMID 9583767. 116. ^ Ockene JK, Barad DH, Cochrane BB, Larson JC, Gass M, Wassertheil-Smoller S, Manson JE, Barnabei VM, Lane DS, Brzyski RG, Rosal MC, Wylie-Rosett J, Hays J (2005). "Symptom experience after discontinuing use of estrogen plus progestin". JAMA 294 (2): 183–93. doi:10.1001/jama.294.2.183. PMID 16014592. 117. ^ Nitzan U, Lichtenberg P (October 23, 2004). "Questionnaire survey on use of placebo". BMJ 329 (7472): 944–6. doi:10.1136/bmj.38236.646678.55. PMC 524103. PMID 15377572. 118. ^ ^a ^b Spiegel D (October 23, 2004). "Placebos in practice: Doctors use them, they work in some conditions, but we don't know how they work". BMJ 329 (7472): 927–8. doi:10.1136/bmj.329.7472.927. PMC 524090. PMID 15499085. 119. ^ ^a ^b Sandler AD, Bodfish JW (2008). "Open-label use of placebos in the treatment of ADHD: a pilot study". Child Care Health Dev 34 (1): 104–10. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00797.x. PMID 18171451. 120. ^ Altunç, U.; Pittler, M. H.; Ernst, E. (2007). "Homeopathy for childhood and adolescence ailments: Systematic review of randomized clinical trials". Mayo Clinic proceedings. Mayo Clinic 82 (1): 69–75. doi:10.4065/82.1.69. PMID 17285788. edit 121. ^ Doctors Struggle With Tougher-Than-Ever Dilemmas: Other Ethical Issues Author: Leslie Kane. 11/11/2010 122. ^ ^a ^b ^c Benedetti F (1996). "The opposite effects of the opiate antagonist naloxone and the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesia". Pain 64 (3): 535–43. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(95)00179-4. PMID 8783319. 123. ^ ^a ^b Levine JD, Gordon NC, Bornstein JC, Fields HL (1979). "Role of pain in placebo analgesia". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 76 (7): 3528–31. Bibcode:1979PNAS...76.3528L. doi:10.1073/pnas.76.7.3528. PMC 383861. PMID 291020. 124. ^ Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M (2002). "Placebo and opioid analgesia: imaging a shared neuronal network". Science 295 (5560): 1737–40. Bibcode:2002Sci...295.1737P. doi:10.1126/science.1067176. PMID 11834781. 125. ^ ^a ^b Levine JD, Gordon NC, Smith R, Fields HL (1981). "Analgesic responses to morphine and placebo in individuals with postoperative pain". Pain 10 (3): 379–89. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(81)90099-3. PMID 7279424. 126. ^ Doongaji DR, Vahia VN, Bharucha MP (1978). "On placebos, placebo responses and placebo responders. (A review of psychological, psychopharmacological and psychophysiological factors)". J Postgrad Med 24 (2): 91–7. PMID 364041. 127. ^ ^a ^b ^c Hoffman GA, Harrington A, Fields HL (2005). "Pain and the placebo: what we have learned". Perspect Biol Med 48 (2): 248–65. doi:10.1353/pbm.2005.0054. PMID 15834197. 128. ^ Zubieta JK, Yau WY, Scott DJ, Stohler CS (2006). "Belief or Need? Accounting for individual variations in the neurochemistry of the placebo effect". Brain Behav Immun 20 (1): 15–26. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2005.08.006. PMID 16242910. > 129. ^ Benedetti F, Arduino C, Costa S, Vighetti S, Tarenzi L, Rainero I, Asteggiano G (2006). "Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer's disease makes analgesic therapies less effective". Pain 121 (1–2): 133–44. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2005.12.016. PMID 16473462. 130. ^ Rheims S, Cucherat M, Arzimanoglou A, Ryvlin P (August 12, 2008). "Greater Response to Placebo in Children Than in Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis in Drug-Resistant Partial Epilepsy". In Klassen, Terry. PLoS Med 5 (8): e166. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0050166. PMC 2504483. PMID 18700812. 131. ^ Furmark T, Appel L, Henningsson S, et al. (December 2008). "A link between serotonin-related gene polymorphisms, amygdala activity, and placebo-induced relief from social anxiety". J. Neurosci. 28 (49): 13066–74. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2534-08.2008. PMID 19052197. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 132. ^ "The Placebo Effect: Not All in Your Head", ScienceNOW Daily News, 2 December 2008 133. ^ "First 'placebo gene' discovered", New Scientist, 03 December 2008 134. ^ Tirrell, Meg (24 October 2012). "Genetics May Help Explain Placebo Effect, Researchers Say". Bloomberg. Retrieved 1 November 2012. 135. ^ Hall, Kathryn; Lembo, Anthony; Kirsch, Irving; Ziogas, Dimitrios; Douaiher, Jeffrey; Jensen, Karin; Conboy, Lisa; Kelley, John et al. (2012). "Catechol-O-Methyltransferase val158met Polymorphism Predicts Placebo Effect in Irritable Bowel Syndrome". PLOS ONE (PLOS) 7 (10): e48135. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...748135H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048135. Retrieved 1 November 2012. |displayauthors= suggested (help) 136. ^ Cousins, Norman (1989). Head First. New York: E. P. Dutton. pp. 229–231. |accessdate= requires |url= (help) 137. ^ Placebo Effect. Cancer.org. Retrieved on 2013-08-25. 138. ^ Levine, J. D.; Gordon, N. C.; Bornstein, J. C.; Fields, H. L. (1979). "Role of pain in placebo analgesia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 76 (7): 3528–3531. doi:10.1073/pnas.76.7.3528. PMC 383861. PMID 291020. edit 139. ^ Amanzio, M.; Benedetti, F. (1999). "Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: Expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems". The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 19 (1): 484–494. PMID 9870976. edit 140. ^ Levine, J. D.; Gordon, N. C. (1984). "Influence of the method of drug administration on analgesic response". Nature 312 (5996): 755–756. doi:10.1038/312755a0. PMID 6514008. edit 141. ^ ^a ^b Kirsch, I. Sapirstein, G. (1998) Listening to Prozac but hearing placebo: A meta-analysis of antidepressant medication. Prevention & Treatment. 1, ArtID 2a abstract 142. ^ Kirsch, I., Deacon, B. J., Huedo-Medina, T. B., Scoboria, A., Moore, T. J., & Johnson, B. T. (2008). Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: A meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLoS Medicine, 5(2). Retrieved from http://medicine.plosjournals.org/perlserv/?request=get-document&doi =10.1371/journal.pmed.0050045 143. ^ Fournier J. C., DeRubeis R. J., Hollon S. D., Dimidjian S., Amsterdam J. D., Shelton R. C. et al. (2010). "Antidepressant Drug Effects and Depression Severity: A Patient-Level Meta-analysis". Journal of the American Medical Association 303 (1): 47–53. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.1943. PMID 20051569. 144. ^ Khan, A, Redding, N, Brown, WA (2008). "The persistence of the placebo response in antidepressant clinical trials". Journal of Psychiatric Research 42 (10): 791–796. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.10.004. PMID 18036616. 145. ^ Khan A, Warner HA, Brown WA (April 2000). "Symptom reduction and suicide risk in patients treated with placebo in antidepressant clinical trials: an analysis of the Food and Drug Administration database". Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57 (4): 311–7. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.311. PMID 10768687. Cite uses deprecated parameters (help) 146. ^ ^a ^b ^c ^d ^e ^f Moerman, Daniel E. (2002). Meaning, Medicine and the Placebo Effect. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521000871. 147. ^ Cho HJ, Hotopf M, Wessely S (2005). "The placebo response in the treatment of chronic fatigue syndrome: A systematic review and meta-analysis". Psychosom Med 67 (2): 301–13. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000156969.76986.e0. PMID 15784798. Retrieved 2008-12-12. 148. ^ Levin FR, Evans SM, Brooks DJ, Kalbag AS, Garawi F, Nunes EV (2006). "Treatment of methadone-maintained patients with adult ADHD: double-blind comparison of methylphenidate, bupropion and placebo". Drug Alcohol Depend 81 (2): 137–48. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.06.012. PMID 16102908. 149. ^ Grandjean P, Guldager B, Larsen IB, Jørgensen PJ, Holmstrup P (1997). "Placebo response in environmental disease. Chelation therapy of patients with symptoms attributed to amalgam fillings". J Occup Environ Med 39 (8): 707–14. PMID 9273873. 150. ^ Schweizer E, Rickels K. (1997). "Placebo response in generalized anxiety: its effect on the outcome of clinical trials". J Clin Psychiatry 58 (Suppl 11): 30–8. PMID 9363046. 151. ^ Piercy MA, Sramek JJ, Kurtz NM, Cutler NR (1996). "Placebo response in anxiety disorders". Ann Pharmacother 30 (9): 1013–9. PMID 8876864. 152. ^ Butler C, Steptoe A (1986). "Placebo responses: an experimental study of psychophysiological processes in asthmatic volunteers". Br J Clin Psychol 25 (3): 173–83. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1986.tb00693.x. PMID 3768575. 153. ^ Kemeny ME, Rosenwasser LJ, Panettieri RA, Rose RM, Berg-Smith SM, Kline JN (2007). "Placebo response in asthma: a robust and objective phenomenon". J Allergy Clin Immunol 119 (6): 1375–81. doi:10.1016/j.jaci.2007.03.016. PMID 17451796. 154. ^ Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Deykin A, Wayne PM, Lasagna LC, Epstein IO, Kirsch I, Wechsler ME (2008). "Do "placebo responders" exist?". Contemp Clin Trials 29 (4): 587–95. doi:10.1016/j.cct.2008.02.002. PMID 18378192. 155. ^ Sandler A (2005). "Placebo effects in developmental disabilities: implications for research and practice". Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 11 (2): 164–70. doi:10.1002/mrdd.20065. PMID 15977316. 156. ^ Sandler AD, Sutton KA, DeWeese J, Girardi MA, Sheppard V, Bodfish JW (1999). "Lack of benefit of a single dose of synthetic human secretin in the treatment of autism and pervasive developmental disorder". N Engl J Med 341 (24): 1801–6. doi:10.1056/NEJM199912093412404. PMID 10588965. 157. ^ Madersbacher S, Marszalek M, Lackner J, Berger P, Schatzl G (2007). "The long-term outcome of medical therapy for BPH". Eur Urol 51 (6): 1522–33. doi:10.1016/j.eururo.2007.03.034. PMID 17416456. 158. ^ Bulik CM, Brownley KA, Shapiro JR (2007). "Diagnosis and management of binge eating disorder". World Psychiatry 6 (3): 142–8. PMC 2174583. PMID 18188431. 159. ^ Sysko R, Walsh BT (2007). "A systematic review of placebo response in studies of bipolar mania". J Clin Psychiatry 68 (8): 1213–7. doi:10.4088/JCP.v68n0807. PMID 17854245. 160. ^ Su C, Lichtenstein GR, Krok K, Brensinger CM, Lewis JD (2004). "A meta-analysis of the placebo rates of remission and response in clinical trials of active Crohn's disease". Gastroenterology 126 (5): 1257–69. doi:10.1053/j.gastro.2004.01.024. PMID 15131785. 161. ^ Loving RT, Kripke DF, Elliott JA, Knickerbocker NC, Grandner MA (2005). "Bright light treatment of depression for older adults ISRCTN55452501". BMC Psychiatry 5: 41. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-5-41. PMC 1298312. PMID 16283925. 162. ^ Egbert LD, Battit GE, Welch CE, Bartlett MK (1964). "Reduction of postoperative pain by encouragement and instruction of patients. A study of doctor-patient rapport". N Engl J Med 270 (16): 825–7. doi:10.1056/NEJM196404162701606. PMID 14108087. 163. ^ Andrews G (2001). "Placebo response in depression: bane of research, boon to therapy". Br J Psychiatry 178 (3): 192–4. doi:10.1192/bjp.178.3.192. PMID 11230026. 164. ^ Moncrieff J, Wessely S, Hardy R (2004). "Active placebos versus antidepressants for depression". In Moncrieff, Joanna. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (1): CD003012. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003012.pub2. PMID 14974002. CD003012. 165. ^ Mearin F, Balboa A, Zárate N, Cucala M, Malagelada JR (1999). "Placebo in functional dyspepsia: symptomatic, gastrointestinal motor, and gastric sensorial responses". Am J Gastroenterol 94 (1): 116–25. doi:10.1111/j.1572-0241.1999.00781.x. PMID 9934741. 166. ^ Niklson I, Edrich P, Verdru P (2006). "Identifying baseline characteristics of placebo responders versus nonresponders in randomized double-blind trials of refractory partial-onset seizures". Epileptic Disord 8 (1): 37–44. PMID 16567324. 167. ^ Kriston L, Harms A, Berner MM (2006). "A meta-regression analysis of treatment effect modifiers in trials with flexible-dose oral sildenafil for erectile dysfunction in broad-spectrum populations". Int J Impot Res 18 (6): 559–65. doi:10.1038/sj.ijir.3901479. PMID 16688210. 168. ^ Moerman DE (2000). "Cultural variations in the placebo effect: ulcers, anxiety, and blood pressure". Medical Anthropology Quarterly 14 (1): 51–72. doi:10.1525/maq.2000.14.1.51. PMID 10812563. 169. ^ Diener HC, Schorn CF, Bingel U, Dodick DW (2008). "The importance of placebo in headache research". Cephalagia 28 (10): 1003–11. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2982.2008.01660.x. PMID 18727647. 170. ^ Archer TP, Leier CV (1992). "Placebo treatment in congestive heart failure". Cardiology 81 (2–3): 125–33. doi:10.1159/000175787. PMID 1286471. 171. ^ Marks R, Koutts J (April 12, 1975). "Topical treatment of recurrent herpes simplex with cytosine arabinoside". Med J Aust 01 (15): 479–80. PMID 1097864. > 172. ^ Asmar R, Safar M, Queneau P (2001). "Evaluation of the placebo effect and reproducibility of blood pressure measurement in hypertension". Am J Hypertens 14 (6 Pt 1): 546–52. doi:10.1016/S0895-7061(00)01286-3. PMID 11411734. 173. ^ Patel SM, Stason WB, Legedza A, Ock SM, Kaptchuk TJ, Conboy L, Canenguez K, Park JK, Kelly E, Jacobson E, Kerr CE, Lembo AJ (2005). "The placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome trials: a meta-analysis". Neurogastroenterol Motil 17 (3): 332–40. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2982.2005.00650.x. PMID 15916620. 174. ^ Pitz M, Cheang M, Bernstein CN (2005). "Defining the predictors of the placebo response in irritable bowel syndrome". Clin Gastroenterol Hepatol 3 (237–47): 237–47. doi:10.1016/S1542-3565(04)00626-3. PMID 15765443. 175. ^ Macedo A, Baños JE, Farré M (2008). "Placebo response in the prophylaxis of migraine: a meta-analysis". Eur J Pain 12 (1): 68–75. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.03.002. PMID 17451980. 176. ^ La Mantia L, Eoli M, Salmaggi A, Milanese C (1996). "Does a placebo-effect exist in clinical trials on multiple sclerosis? Review of the literature". Ital J Neurol Sci 17 (2): 135–9. doi:10.1007/BF02000844. PMID 8797067. 177. ^ Wolf S (1950). "EFFECTS OF SUGGESTION AND CONDITIONING ON THE ACTION OF CHEMICAL AGENTS IN HUMAN SUBJECTS—THE PHARMACOLOGY OF PLACEBOS". J Clin Invest 29 (1): 100–9. doi:10.1172/JCI102225. PMC 439730. PMID 15399519. 178. ^ Zhang Z, Wang Y, Wang Y, Xu F (2008). "Antiemetic placebo: reduce adverse drug interactions between chemotherapeutic agents and antiemetic drugs in cancer patients". Med Hypotheses 70 (3): 551–5. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2007.06.029. PMID 17703892. 179. ^ Shiao SY, Dune LS (2006). "Metaanalyses of acustimulations: effects on nausea and vomiting in postoperative adult patients". Explore (NY) 2 (3): 202–15. doi:10.1016/j.explore.2006.02.005. PMID 16781643. 180. ^ Beecher HK, Keats AS, Mosteller F, Lasagna L (1953). "The effectiveness of oral analgesics (morphine, codeine, acetylsalicylic acid) and the problem of placebo "reactors" and "non-reactors"". J Pharmacol Exp Ther 109 (4): 393–400. PMID 13109703. 181. ^ Baker B, Khaykin Y, Devins G, Dorian P, Shapiro C, Newman D (2003). "Correlates of therapeutic response in panic disorder presenting with palpitations: heart rate variability, sleep, and placebo effect". Can J Psychiatry 48 (6): 381–7. PMID 12894612. 182. ^ de la Fuente-Fernández R, Stoessl AJ (2002). "The placebo effect in Parkinson's disease". Trends Neurosci 25 (6): 302–6. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02181-1. PMID 12086748. 183. ^ Goetz CG, Wuu J, McDermott MP, Adler CH, Fahn S, Freed CR, Hauser RA, Olanow WC, Shoulson I, Tandon PK; Parkinson Study Group, Leurgans S (2008). "Placebo response in Parkinson's disease: comparisons among 11 trials covering medical and surgical interventions". Mov Disord 23 (5): 690–9. doi:10.1002/mds.21894. PMID 18228568. 184. ^ Black DW, Arndt S, Coryell WH, Argo T, Forbush KT, Shaw MC, Perry P, Allen J (2007). "Bupropion in the treatment of pathological gambling: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, flexible-dose study". J Clin Psychopharmacol 27 (2): 143–50. doi:10.1097/01.jcp.0000264985.25109.25. PMID 17414236. 185. ^ Eriksson E, Ekman A, Sinclair S, Sörvik K, Ysander C, Mattson UB, Nissbrandt H (2008). "Escitalopram administered in the luteal phase exerts a marked and dose-dependent effect in premenstrual dysphoric disorder". J Clin Psychopharmacol 28 (2): 195–202. doi:10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181678a28. PMID 18344730. 186. ^ Brockbank J, Gladman D (2002). "Diagnosis and management of psoriatic arthritis". Drugs 62 (17): 2447–57. doi:10.2165/00003495-200262170-00004. PMID 12421102. 187. ^ Pace F, Maconi G, Molteni P, Minguzzi M, Bianchi Porro G (1995). "Meta-analysis of the effect of placebo on the outcome of medically treated reflux esophagitis". Scand J Gastroenterol 30 (2): 101–5. doi:10.3109/00365529509093245. PMID 7732329. 188. ^ Fulda S, Wetter TC (2008). "Where dopamine meets opioids: a meta-analysis of the placebo effect in restless legs syndrome treatment studies". Brain 131 (Pt 4): 902–17. doi:10.1093/brain/awm244. PMID 17932100. 189. ^ Pollo A, Benedetti F (2008). "Placebo response: relevance to the rheumatic diseases". Rheum Dis Clin North Am 34 (2): 331–49. doi:10.1016/j.rdc.2008.04.002. PMID 18638680. 190. ^ Bradford A, Meston C (2007). "Correlates of Placebo Response in the Treatment of Sexual Dysfunction in Women: A Preliminary Report". J Sex Med 4 (5): 1345–51. doi:10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00578.x. PMC 2859204. PMID 17666035. 191. ^ Oosterbaan DB, van Balkom AJ, Spinhoven P, van Dyck R (2001). "The placebo response in social phobia". J Psychopharmacol 15 (3): 199–203. doi:10.1177/026988110101500314. PMID 11565629. 192. ^ Ilnyckyj A, Shanahan F, Anton PA, Cheang M, Bernstein CN (1997). "Quantification of the placebo response in ulcerative colitis". Gastroenterology 112 (6): 1854–8. doi:10.1053/gast.1997.v112.pm9178676. PMID 9178676. 193. ^ Nyirjesy P, Sobel JD, Weitz MV, Leaman DJ, Small MJ, Gelone SP (2001). "Cromolyn cream for recalcitrant idiopathic vulvar vestibulitis: results of a placebo controlled study". Sex Transm Infect 77 (1): 53–7. doi:10.1136/sti.77.1.53. PMC 1758319. PMID 11158692. 194. ^ Kaptchuk TJ, Friedlander E, Kelley JM, Sanchez MN, Kokkotou E, Singer JP, Kowalczykowski M, Miller FG, Kirsch I, Lembo AJ (2009). "Placebos without deception: a randomized controlled trial in irritable bowel syndrome". PLoS ONE 5 (12): e15591. PMC 3008733. PMID 21203519. 195. ^ Whalley B, Hyland ME, Kirsch I (2008). "Consistency of the placebo effect". J Psychosom Res 64 (5): 537–41. 196. ^ Howick J (25 Aug 2009). "Questioning the Methodologic Superiority of 'Placebo' over 'Active' Controlled Trials". Am J Bioethics 9 (9): 34–48. doi:10.1080/15265160903090041. PMID 19998192. 197. ^ Kottow M (21 July 2010). "The improper use of research placebos". J Eval Clin Pract 16 (6): 1041–4. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2753.2009.01246.x. PMID 20663001. 198. ^ Golomb BA, Erickson LC, Koperski S, Sack D, Enkin M, Howick J (2010). "What's in placebos: who knows? Analysis of randomized, controlled trials". Ann Intern Med 153 (8): 532–5. doi:10.1059/0003-4819-153-8-201010190-00010. PMID 20956710. External links[edit] Look up placebo in Wiktionary, the free dictionary. Wikiquote has a collection of quotations related to: Placebo effect * Harvard-wide Program in Placebo Studies & the Therapeutic Encounter * The Placebo Effect at the Skeptic's Dictionary * The Placebo Effect explained on YouTube * Placebos: cracking the code part 1 part 2 BBC/Discovery channel program * "Placebos are getting more effective. Drugmakers are desperate to know why." Wired magazine on the power of the placebo. Retrieved 2010-07-22 * Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects The Lancet (2010) Retrieved from "http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Placebo&oldid=587332645" Categories: * Bioethics * Clinical research * Deception * Experimental psychology * History of medicine * Latin medical phrases * Latin words and phrases * Magical thinking * Medical ethics * Medical terminology * Medicinal chemistry * Mind–body interventions * Pharmacology * Theories * Somatic psychology Hidden categories: * Pages containing cite templates with deprecated parameters * Pages using citations with old-style implicit et al. * Pages using citations with accessdate and no URL * Articles containing potentially dated statements from 2010 * All articles containing potentially dated statements * All articles with unsourced statements * Articles with unsourced statements from May 2012 * Articles with unsourced statements from September 2013 * Wikipedia articles in need of updating from December 2013 * All Wikipedia articles in need of updating Navigation menu Personal tools * Create account * Log in Namespaces * Article * Talk Variants Views * Read * Edit * View history Actions Search ____________________ (BUTTON) Search Navigation * Main page * Contents * Featured content * Current events * Random article * Donate to Wikipedia Interaction * Help * About Wikipedia * Community portal * Recent changes * Contact page Tools * What links here * Related changes * Upload file * Special pages * Permanent link * Page information * Data item * Cite this page Print/export * Create a book * Download as PDF * Printable version Languages * العربية * Azərbaycanca * Български * Bosanski * Català * Čeština * Dansk * Deutsch * Eesti * Ελληνικά * Español * Esperanto * Euskara * فارسی * Français * Gaeilge * Galego * 한국어 * Hrvatski * Ido * Bahasa Indonesia * Italiano * עברית * Basa Jawa * ქართული * Latviešu * Lietuvių * Magyar * Македонски * مازِرونی * Nederlands * 日本語 * Norsk bokmål * Polski * Português * Română * Русский * Shqip * Simple English * Slovenčina * Slovenščina * Српски / srpski * Suomi * Svenska * தமிழ் * ไทย * Türkçe * Українська * Winaray * 中文 * * Edit links * This page was last modified on 23 December 2013 at 05:47. * Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization. * Privacy policy * About Wikipedia * Disclaimers * Contact Wikipedia * Developers * Mobile view * Wikimedia Foundation * Powered by MediaWiki § The Skeptic's Dictionary A Collection of Strange Beliefs, Amusing Deceptions, and Dangerous Delusions * Home * What's New? * Newsletter * Feedback * FAQ & Interviews * Site Map Contents 1. H. K. Beecher 2. false placebo 3. Hróbjartsson and Götzsche 4. placebos and pain relief 5. "it's all in your mind" 6. process-of-treatment belief 7. ethical dilemma 8. the genetic connection 9. are placebos dangerous? 10. list of other Skeptic's Dictionary entries of relevance to the placebo effect 11. further reading --books & articles --websites & blogs --news stories Topical Indexes * Alternative Medicine * Critical Thinking * Cryptozoology * ETs & UFOs * Frauds, Hoaxes, Conspiracies * Junk Science * Logic & Perception * New Age * Paranormal * Science & Philosophy * Supernatural * Who Am I? * Other Writings * Unnatural Acts blog * Skeptimedia * Mass Media Funk * Mass Media Bunk * What's the Harm? * Newsletter Archives * Internet Bunk * Too good to be true * Critical Thinking * Skeptical Essays * Book Reviews * Suburban Myths * In Memoriam Other Resources * Mysteries and Science * Recommended Books * For Teachers * A Skeptic's Halloween * Editor's Notes * Skeptical Links * Get involved * Future Topics? * Permission to print * Site Statistics From Abracadabra to Zombies | View All * a * b * c * d * e * f * g * h * i * j * k * l * m * n * o * p * q * r * s * t * u * v * w * x * y * z placebo effect "The physician's belief in the treatment and the patient's faith in the physician exert a mutually reinforcing effect; the result is a powerful remedy that is almost guaranteed to produce an improvement and sometimes a cure." -- Petr Skrabanek and James McCormick, Follies and Fallacies in Medicine, p. 13. The placebo effect is the measurable, observable, or felt improvement in health or behavior not attributable to a medication or invasive treatment that has been administered. The placebo effect is not mind over matter; it is not mind-body medicine. 'The placebo effect' has become a catchall term for a positive change in health not attributable to medication or treatment. As is explained below, the change can be due to many things, such as regression to the mean, spontaneous improvement, reduction of stress, misdiagnosis in the first place, subject expectancy, classical conditioning, etc. A placebo (Latin for "I shall please") is a pharmacologically inert substance (such as saline solution or a starch tablet) that seems to produce an effect similar to what would be expected of a pharmacologically active substance (such as an antibiotic). By extension, "fake" surgery and "fake" therapies are considered placebos. The idea of the placebo in modern times originated with H. K. Beecher. He evaluated 15 clinical trials concerned with different diseases and found that 35% of 1,082 patients were satisfactorily relieved by a placebo alone ("The Powerful Placebo," 1955). Other studies have since calculated the placebo effect as being even greater than Beecher claimed. For example, studies have shown that placebos are effective in 50 or 60 percent of subjects with certain conditions, e.g., "pain, depression, some heart ailments, gastric ulcers and other stomach complaints."* And, as effective as the new psychotropic drugs seem to be in the treatment of various brain disorders, some researchers maintain that there is not adequate evidence from studies to prove that the new drugs are more effective than placebos. Beecher started a wave of studies aimed at understanding how something (improvement in health) could be produced by nothing (the inactive placebo). Unfortunately, many of the studies have not been of particularly high quality and have assumed that any measured improvement was caused by the placebo. In fact, it has been argued by Kienle and Kiene (1997) that, contrary to what Beecher claimed, a reanalysis of his data found "no evidence of any placebo effect in any of the studies cited by him." The reported improvements in heath were real but were due to other things that produced "false impressions of placebo effects." The reanalysis of Beecher's data claims that the improvements were due to: Spontaneous improvement, fluctuation of symptoms, regression to the mean, additional treatment, conditional switching of placebo treatment, scaling bias, irrelevant response variables, answers of politeness, experimental subordination, conditioned answers, neurotic or psychotic misjudgment, psychosomatic phenomena, misquotation, etc. What the reanalysis shows is that there are a number of factors that can affect many treatments and the evaluation of those treatments, making it very difficult to be sure just what it is about an intervention that produces improvement or perceived improvement. We must also consider "artifacts such as the natural history of a disease (that is, the tendency for people to get better or worse during the course of an illness irrespective of any treatment at all), the fact that people behave differently when they are participating in an experiment than when they are not, a desire to please the experimental staff by providing socially desirable answers..." (Bausell 2007: 27), and a host of other factors unrelated to the pill we are administering and independently of any mechanism that we believe is producing any observed effects. In May 2001, The New England Journal of Medicine published an article that called into question the validity of the placebo effect. "Is the Placebo Powerless? An Analysis of Clinical Trials Comparing Placebo with No Treatment" by Danish researchers Asbjørn Hróbjartsson and Peter C. Götzsche "found little evidence in general that placebos had powerful clinical effects." Their meta-analysis of 114 studies found that "compared with no treatment, placebo had no significant effect on binary outcomes, regardless of whether these outcomes were subjective or objective. For the trials with continuous outcomes, placebo had a beneficial effect, but the effect decreased with increasing sample size, indicating a possible bias related to the effects of small trials." (Most of the studies evaluated by Hróbjartsson and Götzsche were small: for 82 of the studies the median size was 27 and for the other 32 studies the median was 51.) "The high levels of placebo effect which have been repeatedly reported in many articles, in our mind are the result of flawed research methodology," said Dr. Hróbjartsson, professor of medical philosophy and research methodology at the University of Copenhagen.* Typical of the kind of flawed research methodology Hróbjartsson is referring to would be that of surgeon J. Bruce Moseley who performed fake knee surgery on eight of ten patients. (Fake surgery involves making an incision on the knee and stitching it up.) Six months after the surgery all the patients were satisfied customers. Rather than conclude that the patients didn't need surgery or that the surgery was useless because in time the patients would have healed on their own, he and others concluded that the healing of the eight who did not have surgery was due to the placebo effect, while the two who had real surgery were better because of having had the operation. Irving Kirsch and Guy Sapirstein have been accused of making the same kind of methodological error in their controversial meta-analysis that found that anti-depressants work by the placebo effect, rather than that anti-depressants are unnecessary and useless. One more example should suffice to make the point that better designs of placebo studies are needed. Forty years ago, a young Seattle cardiologist named Leonard Cobb conducted a unique trial of a procedure then commonly used for angina, in which doctors made small incisions in the chest and tied knots in two arteries to try to increase blood flow to the heart. It was a popular technique—90 percent of patients reported that it helped—but when Cobb compared it with placebo surgery in which he made incisions but did not tie off the arteries, the sham operations proved just as successful. The procedure, known as internal mammary ligation, was soon abandoned ("The Placebo Prescription" by Margaret Talbot, New York Times Magazine, January 9, 2000).* Did Cobb show that this kind of surgery works by the placebo effect? Or did he show that the surgery was unnecessary because most of the patients would have healed on their own if nothing had been done? To rule out the natural history of a disease or regression to the mean, many researchers have used a third control group—those who receive no treatment at all. If the placebo group shows better results than the group getting nothing, then surely the placebo is effective. Hróbjartsson and Götzsche think most of these studies, too, are flawed, mainly due to having samples that are too small or due to patients who make reports aimed at pleasing the researcher. After the publication of the Hróbjartsson and Götzsche study, Dr. John C. Bailar III said in an editorial that accompanied the study: "The shoe is on the other foot now. The people who claim there are placebo effects are going to have to show it." The need, he said, is for large, rigorously designed studies that clearly define and measure effects of drugs and therapies versus placebos versus no intervention at all. These studies will have to clearly distinguish objective measurements (such as blood pressure, cholesterol levels, etc.) and subjective measurements (such as reports of pain or evaluative sensory observations by researchers, e.g., "I can see your tumor is smaller" or "I can see you are not as depressed as before"). The kind of study called for by Dr. Bailar has been done and several such studies are reviewed in chapter nine of R. Barker Bausell's Snake Oil Science (2007): "How We Know That the Placebo Effect Exists." One in particular is worth reviewing here. It was published in the Journal Pain two months after the Hróbjartsson and Götzsche article. "Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance" was the work of Antonella Pollo et al. and demonstrated that placebos can help people with serious pain. The following is from their abstract: Thoracotomized patients were treated with buprenorphine [a powerful pain reliever] on request for 3 consecutive days, together with a basal intravenous infusion of saline solution. However, the symbolic meaning of this basal infusion was changed in three different groups of patients. The first group was told nothing about any analgesic effect (natural history). The second group was told that the basal infusion was either a powerful painkiller or a placebo (classic double-blind administration). The third group was told that the basal infusion was a potent painkiller (deceptive administration). Therefore, whereas the analgesic treatment was exactly the same in the three groups, the verbal instructions about the basal infusion differed. The placebo effect of the saline basal infusion was measured by recording the doses of buprenorphine requested over the three-days treatment. We found that the double-blind group showed a reduction of buprenorphine requests compared to the natural history group. However, this reduction was even larger in the deceptive administration group. Overall, after 3 days of placebo infusion, the first group received 11.55 mg of buprenorphine, the second group 9.15 mg, and the third group 7.65 mg. Despite these dose differences, analgesia was the same in the three groups. These results indicate that different verbal instructions about certain and uncertain expectations of analgesia produce different placebo analgesic effects, which in turn trigger a dramatic change of behaviour leading to a significant reduction of opioid intake. The patients who thought their IV contained a powerful pain reliever required 34% less of the analgesic than the patients who weren't told anything about their IV and 16% less than the patients who were told the IV could be either a powerful pain killer or a placebo. Each group got exactly the same amount of pain killer but their requests for the analgesic differed dramatically. The only significant difference among the three groups was the set of verbal instructions about the basal infusion. The study was too short for the differences to be explained by the natural history of recovery, regression, or any of the other alternatives found by Hróbjartsson and Götzsche. Several things are worth noting about this experiment. The setting involves treatment being provided by medical personnel in a medical facility. This kind of setting usually involves a strong desire for recovery or relief on the part of the patient, as well as a belief that the treatment will be effective. The different verbal instructions about the basal IV would lead to different expectations. Belief, motivation, and expectation are essential to some forms of the placebo effect. Together, they are referred to as the subject-expectancy effect. Classical conditioning and suggestion by an authoritative healer seem to be triggering mechanisms for [DEL: the :DEL] this form of placebo effect (Bausell 2007: 131). the psychological hypothesis: it's all in your mind Some believe the placebo effect is purely psychological. Irving Kirsch, a psychologist at the University of Connecticut, believes that the effectiveness of Prozac and similar drugs may be attributed almost entirely to the placebo effect. He and Guy Sapirstein analyzed 19 clinical trials of antidepressants and concluded that the expectation of improvement, not adjustments in brain chemistry, accounted for 75 percent of the drugs' effectiveness (Kirsch 1998). "The critical factor," says Kirsch, "is our beliefs about what's going to happen to us. You don't have to rely on drugs to see profound transformation." In an earlier study, Sapirstein analyzed 39 studies, done between 1974 and 1995, of depressed patients treated with drugs, psychotherapy, or a combination of both. He found that 50 percent of the drug effect is due to the placebo response. A person's beliefs and hopes about a treatment, combined with their suggestibility, may have a significant biochemical effect, however. Sensory experience and thoughts can affect neurochemistry. The body's neurochemical system affects and is affected by other biochemical systems, including the hormonal and immune systems. Thus, it is consistent with current knowledge that a person's hopeful attitude and beliefs may be very important to their physical well-being and recovery from injury or illness. But it does not follow from this fact that if the patient has hope will she recover. Nor does it follow from this fact that if a person is not hopeful she will not recover. The psychological explanation seems to be the one most commonly believed. Perhaps this is why many people are dismayed when they are told that the effective drug they are taking is a placebo. This makes them think that their problem is "all in their mind" and that there is really nothing wrong with them. Yet, there are too many studies that have found objective improvements in health after being given placebos to support the notion that the placebo effect is entirely psychological. Doctors in one study successfully eliminated warts by painting them with a brightly colored, inert dye and promising patients the warts would be gone when the color wore off. In a study of asthmatics, researchers found that they could produce dilation of the airways by simply telling people they were inhaling a bronchodilator, even when they weren't. Patients suffering pain after wisdom-tooth extraction got just as much relief from a fake application of ultrasound as from a real one, so long as both patient and therapist thought the machine was on. Fifty-two percent of the colitis patients treated with placebo in 11 different trials reported feeling better -- and 50 percent of the inflamed intestines actually looked better when assessed with a sigmoidoscope ("The Placebo Prescription" by Margaret Talbot, New York Times Magazine, January 9, 2000).* It is unlikely that such effects are purely psychological, though I must admit that I don't find the expression 'purely psychological' very precise or clear. In fact, Martina Amanzio et al. (2001) demonstrated that "at least part of the physiological basis for the placebo effect is opioid in nature" (Bausell 2007: 160). We can be conditioned to release such chemical substances as endorphins, catecholamines, cortisol, and adrenaline. One reason, therefore, that people report pain relief from both acupuncture and sham acupuncture [DEL: is :DEL] may be that both are placebos that stimulate the opioid system. the process-of-treatment belief Another popular belief is that a process of treatment that involves showing attention, care, affection, etc., to the patient/subject, a process that is encouraging and hopeful, may itself trigger physical reactions in the body which promote healing. According to Dr. Walter A. Brown, a psychiatrist at Brown University: ...there is certainly data that suggest that just being in the healing situation accomplishes something. Depressed patients who are merely put on a waiting list for treatment do not do as well as those given placebos. And—this is very telling, I think—when placebos are given for pain management, the course of pain relief follows what you would get with an active drug. The peak relief comes about an hour after it's administered, as it does with the real drug, and so on. If placebo analgesia was the equivalent of giving nothing, you'd expect a more random pattern ("The Placebo Prescription" by Margaret Talbot, New York Times Magazine, January 9, 2000).* Dr. Brown and others believe that the placebo effect is mainly or purely physical and due to physical changes that promote healing or feeling better. So, what is the explanatory mechanism for the placebo effect? Some think it is the process of administering it. It is thought that the touching, the caring, the attention, and other interpersonal communication that is part of the controlled study process (or the therapeutic setting), along with the hopefulness and encouragement provided by the experimenter/healer, affect the mood, expectations, and beliefs of the subject, which in turn triggers physical changes such as release of endorphins, catecholamines, cortisol, or adrenaline. The process reduces stress by providing hope or reducing uncertainty about what treatment to take or what the outcome will be. The reduction in stress prevents or slows down further harmful physical changes from occurring. The healing situation provokes a conditioned response. The patient's been healed before by the doctor (or thinks she's been healed before by the doctor) and expects to be healed again. the genetic connection Because the placebo effect shows profound variability among individuals, some researchers have looked for evidence of a genetic predisposition to susceptibility to placebo effects. Andrew Leuchter et al. postulated that placebos act through central reward pathways modulated by monoamines, which are under strong genetic control. Their findings "support the hypothesis that genetic polymorphisms modulating monoaminergic tone are related to degree of placebo responsiveness in major depressive disorder."* The researchers stressed that genetics is not the sole explanation for a placebo response, which is likely to be influenced by several biological and psychosocial factors.* the ethical dilemma The power of the placebo effect has led to an ethical dilemma. One should not deceive other people, but one should relieve the pain and suffering of one's patients. Should one use deception to benefit one's patients? Is it unethical for a doctor to knowingly prescribe a placebo without informing the patient? If informing the patient reduces the effectiveness of the placebo, is some sort of deception warranted in order to benefit the patient? Some doctors think it is justified to use a placebo in those types of cases where a strong placebo effect has been shown and where distress is an aggravating factor.* Others think it is always wrong to deceive the patient and that informed consent requires that the patient be told that a treatment is a placebo treatment. Others, especially complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) practitioners, don't even want to know whether a treatment is a placebo or not. Their attitude is that as long as the treatment is effective, who cares if it a placebo? This attitude is changing, however, and it is now common to find defenders of CAM admit that CAM is placebo medicine and go on to claim that that's why CAM is good medicine! While it may be unethical to knowingly package, prescribe, or sell placebos as magical cures, the CAM folks seem to think they are ethical because they really believe in their chi, meridians, yin, yang, prana, vata, pitta, kapha, auras, chakras, energies, spirits, succussion, natural herbs, water with precise and selective memory, subluxations, cranial and vertebral manipulations, douches and irrigations, body maps, divinities, and various unobservable processes that allegedly carry out all sorts of magical analgesic and curative functions. are placebos dangerous? While skeptics may reject faith, prayer and "alternative" medical practices such as bioharmonics, chiropractic, and homeopathy, such practices may not be without their salutary effects. Clearly, they can't cure cancer or repair a punctured lung, and they might not even prolong life by giving hope and relieving distress as is sometimes thought. But administering placebo therapies does involve interacting with the patient in a caring, attentive way, and this can provide some measure of comfort. However, to those who say "what difference does it make why something works, as long as it seems to work" I reply that it is likely that there is something that works even better and might even be cheaper. Worse, some people might seek out an alternative healer for a serious disorder that isn't affected by the CAM treatment but could be relieved or cured by scientific medicine. Furthermore, placebos may not always be beneficial or harmless. John Dodes notes: Patients can become dependent on nonscientific practitioners who employ placebo therapies. Such patients may be led to believe they're suffering from imagined "reactive" hypoglycemia, nonexistent allergies and yeast infections, dental filling amalgam "toxicity," or that they're under the power of qi or extraterrestrials. And patients can be led to believe that diseases are only amenable to a specific type of treatment from a specific practitioner (The Mysterious Placebo by John E. Dodes, Skeptical Inquirer, Jan/Feb 1997). In other words, the placebo can be an open door to malfeasance. R. Barker Bausell speculates that since complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) practitioners' greatest asset is their nourishment of hope (2007: 294), "such therapies may be engendering nothing more than the expectation that they will reduce pain by elaborate explanations, promises, and ceremonies" (p. 149). Packaging placebos is big business and is likely to get even bigger. The only thing that could slow down CAM atavism would be the sudden appearance of horrible side effects issuing from treatments like aura cleansings or homeopathic douches. I'd say that there's about as much chance of that happening as there is of John Edward or James Van Praagh announcing to an audience that a spirit is telling him that one of the paying customers is an axe murderer. See also conditioning, confirmation bias, control study, communal reinforcement, magical thinking, nocebo, Occam's razor, post hoc fallacy, regressive fallacy, selective thinking, self-deception, subjective validation, testimonials, and wishful thinking. For examples of beliefs deeply affected by the placebo effect see the following entries in The Skeptic's Dictionary: * acupuncture * alphabiotics * angel therapy * animal quacker * anthroposophic medicine * applied kinesiology * aromatherapy * astrotherapy * aura therapy * Ayurvedic medicine * Bach's flower therapy * bio-ching * bioharmonics * chelation therapy * chiropractic * complex homeopathy * coning (ear candling) * Consegrity * craniosacral therapy * DHEA * dolphin-assisted therapy * Dr. Fritz - "energy healing" * Eye Movement Desensitization Reprocessing (EMDR) * facilitated communication * faith healing * healing touch * homeopathy * hypnotherapy * iridology * joy touch * magnet therapy * microacupuncture * naturopathy * osteopathy * prayer * psychic surgery * reflexology * reiki * therapeutic touch * trepanation * urine therapy _________________ Unlike EMC Certification and mcitp training program, our 1Y0-A24 and ccna security courses provide you quick success in first try of Pass4sure 312-50v7. -- further reading books and articles Amanzio Martina et al. (2001). Response variability to analgesics: a role for non-specific activation of endogenous opioids. Pain. Feb 15;90(3):205-15. Bausell, R. Barker. (2007). Snake Oil Science: The Truth about Complementary and Alternative Medicine Oxford. Dodes, John E. (1997). The Mysterious Placebo. Skeptical Inquirer. Engel, Linda W. et al. The Science of the Placebo - Toward an Interdisciplinary Research Agenda ( BMJ Books, 2002). Fisher, Seymour and Roger P. Greenberg. eds. From Placebo to Panacea: Putting Psychiatric Drugs to the Test (John Wiley and Sons, 1997). Hart, Carol. The Mysterious Placebo Effect. Modern Drug Discovery July/August 1999 Hróbjartsson, Asbjørn and Peter C. Götzsche. "Is the Placebo Powerless? An Analysis of Clinical Trials Comparing Placebo with No Treatment," The New England Journal of Medicine, May 24, 2001 (Vol. 344, No. 21). Abstract. Harrington, Anne. ed. The Placebo Effect : An Interdisciplinary Exploration (Harvard University Press, 1999). Jerome, Lawrence E. Crystal Power - The Ultimate Placebo Effect (Amherst, NY: Prometheus, 1996). Kaptchuk, Ted J. et al. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. British Medical Journal, May 2008 Kirsch, Irving , Ph.D. and Guy Sapirstein, Ph.D. "Listening to Prozac but Hearing Placebo: A Meta-Analysis of Antidepressant Medication" Prevention & Treatment, Volume 1, June 1998. Moerman, Daniel E. (2002). Meaning, Medicine and the 'Placebo Effect'. Cambridge University Press. Ogelsby, Dr. Paul. The Caring Physician : The Life of Dr. Francis W. Peabody (Harvard University Press, 1991). Price, D. D. et al. (1999). An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm. Pain, Volume 83, Number 2. Price, Donald D. et al. (2005). Conditioning, expectation, and desire for relief in placebo analgesia. Seminars in Pain Medicine. Volume 3, Issue 1. Abstract. Skrabanek, Petr, and James McCormick, (1990). Follies & Fallacies in Medicine. Prometheus. Sternberg, Esther M. and Philip W. Gold. "The Mind-Body Interaction in Disease," Scientific American," special issue "Mysteries of the Mind," (January 1997). Shapiro, Arthur K. and Elaine Shapiro. The Powerful Placebo: From Ancient Priest to Modern Physician (Johns Hopkins University Press, 1997). Stanovich, Keith E. How to Think Straight About Psychology, 3rd ed., (New York: Harper Collins, 1992). Thompson, W. Grant. (2005). The Placebo Effect and Health: Combining Science and Compassionate Care. Prometheus. Watts, Geoff. The power of nothing. New Scientist White, Leonard, Bernard Tursky and Gary Schwartz. Placebo: Theory Research, and Mechanisms, ed. (New York: Guilford Press, 1985). websites & blogs Several posts by Orac at Respectful Insolence and his evil twin David Gorski at Science-Based Medicine go into great detail about how CAM defenders have co-opted so-called placebo medicine. Placebo effects are "proof" that God [sic] exists? Yes, someone has made this argument and posted it on the Huffington Post Adventures in defending science-based medicine in cancer journals: Energy chelation Does thinking make it so? CAM, placebos, and the new paternalism Credulous reporting on placebo effects strikes again Also, Stephen Novella, M.D., has posted this piece: American Headache Society Recommends Placebos for Migraine The placebo in question is acupuncture. Transcript of BRAIN SCIENCE PODCAST With Ginger Campbell, MD Episode #77 Originally Aired 9/19 /2011 Interview with Fabrizio Benedetti, MD, PhD, Author of Placebo Effects: Understanding the Mechanisms in Health and Disease, and The Patient’s Brain: The Neuroscience behind the Doctor-Patient Relationship What's in a placebo? Mike Adams certainly doesn't know "...the real difference between skeptics and pseudoskeptics is that skeptics base their skepticism on science and evidence. Pseudoskeptics like Mike Adams do not; rather, they base it in ideology. What’s In Placebos? by Steven Novella, M.D. "A new study in the Annals of Internal Medicine reviews clinical trials over the last two years. They found that only 8.2% of clinical trials with a placebo pill as a control specifically disclosed the placebo content. Meanwhile 26.7% of trials involving injections and procedures disclosed the precise nature of the treatment." To see the abstract of the study, click here. Prescribing Placebos by R. T. Carroll My review of Snake Oil Science Placebo Effects Revisited by Steven Novella Existing evidence strongly suggests that placebo effects are mostly comprised of bias in reporting and observation and non-specific effects. There is no measurable physiological benefit from placebo interventions for any objective outcome. There is a measured benefit for some subjective outcomes (mostly pain, nausea, asthma, and phobias), but the wide variation in effect size suggests this is due to trial design (and therefore bias) rather than a real effect. The Placebo Effect by Steven Novella "Placebo Effect Accounts For Fifty Percent Of Improvement In Depressed Patients Taking Antidepressants" by the American Psychological Association Not treating - a neglected option by Harriet Hall Ben Goldacre on the placebo effect news stories Irritable Bowel Syndrome: Placebo Works Even if Patients Know "Incredibly, according to a new study of patients with irritable bowel syndrome, the placebo effect, even when patients were in on the secret, worked almost as well as the leading medication on the market....After three weeks. the placebo group reported adequate symptom relief at double the rate of the group told to do nothing (59 percent vs. 35 percent). And those results are about as good as the leading irritable bowel syndrome drugs on the market. Researchers sounded the usual cautionary notes. The study was small. It's not clear what it would mean for other conditions and more research is needed." Even so, the study supports the notion that the leading IBS medication is a placebo. Since many cases of IBS are stress-related, it makes sense that placebos would work well here if the process of administering them reduces stress. There's a lot the article doesn't cover. For a thorough analysis, see Respectful Insolence's take on this study. Or, better yet, look at the article on PLoS ONE: Placebos without Deception: A Randomized Controlled Trial in Irritable Bowel Syndrome The magic cure "There are limits to even the strongest placebo effect. No simulation could set a broken arm, of course, or clear a blocked artery. As a rule, placebos appear to affect symptoms rather than underlying diseases--although sometimes, as in the case of depression or irritable bowel syndrome, there’s no meaningful distinction between the two. Moreover, placebos have often received undue credit for recovery that might have occurred anyway. Indeed, the effect is famously difficult to identify, measure, and even coherently define. There is debate about the magnitude of the response, with some calling it modest at best, and opposing the idea of using placebos clinically." “Connecting with the patient, rapport, empathy . . . that few extra minutes is not just icing on the cake. It has biology.” Placebos: Pretty Good for Depression A meta-analysis published in JAMA confirmed what several other studies have found: anti-depressants are placebos for all but the severely depressed. The authors of the study concluded: the average benefit of antidepressant medication in patients with mild or moderate symptoms of depression is minimal. "The mere act of seeing a doctor, discussing symptoms and learning about depression probably triggers the improvements many patients experience while on medication."* (See next two entries, please.) Before You Quit Antidepressants ... The study is a so-called meta-analysis — not a fresh clinical trial....For the recent analysis in the journal, the authors identified 23 studies (out of several hundred clinical trials) that met their criteria for inclusion. Of those 23, they could get access to data on only 6, with a total of 718 subjects....That is not many studies if your goal is to answer a broad question about the efficacy of antidepressants as a class. Study shows antidepressants useless for mild to moderate depression? Not exactly. - Science-Based Medicine - "...the paper included only 3 studies of an SSRI. How did the authors whittle down thousands of papers on SSRI effectiveness to only 3?...by using questionable exclusion criteria, the authors accessed only 3 clinically relevant studies (the Paxil studies), involving only one SSRI. It is not clear that these studies are representative of existing studies on SSRIs, or even if they can be generalized to other SSRIs." (See also Science by Press Release.) MEDIA WATCH Antidepressant-Placebo Debate in the Media Balanced Coverage or Placebo Hype? German study on the placebo effect hyped by mediaThis is about a report in Science. The study had only 15 participants, all men. It involved doing an fMRI of their spinal columns while applying different degrees of heat to their arms, which had been treated with an analgesic or a placebo. Some of these stories seem to think the study is groundbreaking because it shows that the placebo effect is not all in the head. We already knew that from numerous studies. Some claim that the study proves spinal inhibition is one of the mechanisms by which the placebo effect reduces pain. No, it doesn't. The study is too small to prove anything. It needs to be replicated with larger groups and with some extensions to make sure the fMRI readings are caused by the placebo and indicate what the researchers think they indicate. A Doctor for Disease, a Shaman for the Soul Mercy Medical Center in Merced figured it was healthier to bring them in rather than shut them out, so it invited the many Hmong shamans who dwell in California's central valley to learn about medicine in exchange for allowing the shamans to treat spirits in the hospital. Superstition and science work hand-in-hand in more than 60 hospitals in the United States in an effort to accommodate cultural beliefs and break down barriers that keep many people from seeking treatment for their serious health problems. Placebos Are Getting More Effective. Drugmakers Are Desperate to Know Why. By Steve Silberman. Wired. 24 Aug 2009. [The article is worth reading but the blinking ad on the right might drive you away. Shrink your viewing window to block out the annoying ad.] First, the facts: an increasing number of new medications are unable to beat sugar pills in RCTs (randomized control studies); trials done in different cultures get different placebo responses; and those of us subjected to constant advertising of pills for every ailment under the sun packaged with stories from satisfied customers are being conditioned to expect relief for our ailments from a pill. It is, of course, possible that the effectiveness of new drugs versus placebos is decreasing because the clinical trials are getting better or the new medications are less effective. There have been studies that have found that certain persons are more susceptible to placebos than others. What is intriguing is that there has been very little study on the cultural influences that affect placebo response. More intriguing is the role of cultural conditioning on susceptibility to placebo effect. Though some laugh at the idea of the placebo effect increasing, it is possible that through social conditioning more and more people could become more affected by placebo medicine (including the theater, the suggestions, and all the other things that are lumped together under the 'placebo effect'). Placebo medicine to be offered by NHS in Northern Ireland Homeopathy, acupuncture, and reflexology are among a range of placebo therapies a new trial concludes should be provided by the NHS. "As many as 81% of patients receiving the treatments on referral from their GP reported improvements in their physical health, and 79% in their mental health." It will save money, too. First 'placebo gene' discovered The placebo effect is the healing force of nature by G. Zajicek The Placebo Prescription - New York Times Magazine 1/09/2000 Sham Surgery Returns as a Research Tool by Sheryl Gay Stolberg, New York Times 4/25/1999 A surprisingly potent placebo: face time with me by Dr. Michael Evans Researchers Debunk Placebo Effect, Saying It's Only a Myth by Gina Kolata (NY Times) Half of Doctors Routinely Prescribe Placebos Placebos Recommended Routinely By Doctors Scientists See How Placebo Effect Eases Pain - Scientific American "Against Depression, a Sugar Pill Is Hard to Beat Placebos Improve Mood, Change Brain Chemistry in Majority of Trials of Antidepressants" by Shankar Vedantam Last updated 29-Dec-2013 ____________________ Search ( ) Web (*) Skepdic.com Google Listen to my podcast on the placebo Books by R. T. Carroll cover The Critical Thinker's Dictionary The Skeptic's Dictionary Ordering information Becoming a Critical Thinker OTHER LANGUAGES * Dutch Dutch voor kinderen * French * German * Greek * Hungarian * Icelandic * Indonesian * Indonesian for kids * Italian * Japanese * Korean * Portuguese Russian * Slovak * Spanish Swedish Turkish Print versions available in Dutch, Russian, Japanese, and Korean. larrow.gif (1051 bytes) Leonora Piper | placebo jewelry rarrow.gif (1048 bytes) © Copyright 1994-2013 Robert T. Carroll * This page was designed by Cristian Popa. setstats § #How Stuff Works Feature Articles HowStuffWorks ICRA labels [discovery-attrib.gif] ____________________ [transparent.png]-Submit [facebook-20x20.png] IFRAME: //www.facebook.com/plugins/likebox.php?href=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.facebook.c om%2FHowStuffWorks&width=300&height=258&colorscheme=light&show_faces=tr ue&border_color=%23ffffff&stream=false&header=false [twitter-20x20_1.png] IFRAME: http://platform.twitter.com/widgets/follow_button.html?screen_name=HowS tuffWorks&lang=en&show_count=true * Adventure * Auto * Culture * Entertainment * Home & Garden * Money * Science * Tech * Video * Shows * Blogs * Quizzes * Games * Random Article * Engineering * * Environmental Science * * Forces of Nature * * Innovation * * Life Science * * Military * * Physical Science * * Dictionary * * Science Vs. Myth * * Space * * Transportation * * Zoology * Home / * Science / * Life Science / * Inside the Mind / * The Human Brain More Stuff Like This Top 10 Scientific Discoveries of 2013 Top 10 Scientific Discoveries of 2013 10 Studies Connecting Completely Bizarre Things 10 Studies Connecting Completely Bizarre Things Puzzle: Studying DNA Puzzle: Studying DNA How the Placebo Effect Works by Shanna Freeman [hsw-share-fb.png] Page 1 Introduction to How the Placebo Effect Works 2 Placebos: Is It All in Your Head? 3 Placebos in Research 4 Prescribing Placebos [hsw-article-keep-reading-short-button.png] Drug Image Gallery Drug Image Gallery Is this what you came for? Get facts about drugs with more drug pictures. ©iStockphoto.com/VisualField Drugs and the Body * Why is it so hard to quit smoking? * 10 Weird Prescription Drug Side Effects * DiscoveryHealth.com: Pros/Cons of Placebos Most of us only go to the doctor when we're not feeling well. You read old magazines in the waiting room, pony up your insurance company's co-pay, answer a stream of seemingly endless questions, and put up with being poked and prodded. And for enduring all of this, you expect something in return. More often than not, that something is a piece of paper with a prescription scrawled on it. You don't usually worry much about what's in the medicine that the doctor has prescribed, as long as it gets you feeling like yourself again. You trust that the doctor knows what's best. But how much does that trust play into the healing process? What if, after filling that prescription and taking it faithfully, you found out that the medicine your doctor had given you wasn't proven to make you feel better? And yet you did get better. You expected to get well after taking those pills, so you did. That's the gist of the placebo effect. It's what happens when a person takes a medication that he or she perceives will help, although it actually has no proven therapeutic effect for his or her particular condition. The medicine or treatment itself is known as a placebo, from Latin for "I will please." There are a few different types of placebos. They may be pharmacologically inert, meaning that they contain no active ingredients. These types of placebos often contain basic ingredients like sugar (hence the term "sugar pill"). Medications that do have active ingredients but aren't proven to work on the patient's particular condition can also be placebos. There have even been placebos in the form of surgery, injections and other types of medical therapies. Some people believe that complementary and alternative medicine count as placebos, too. Placebos have been shown to work in about 30 percent of patients, and they've been used by doctors for ages. In fact, they were often the only thing that a doctor could offer to relieve suffering, other than his or her attention and support. Some researchers believe that placebos simply evoke a psychological response. The act of taking them gives you an improved sense of well-being. However, recent research indicates that placebos may also bring about a physical response. In light of this, some people don't see anything wrong with a doctor prescribing a placebo. After all, he or she is doing it to help the patient. But others see the practice not only as harmful, but unethical, deceptive and possibly even illegal. Although we've long known that placebos can work, we've only recently started to figure out how and why. On the next page, we'll look at some theories behind the therapeutic effects of placebos. [embedded-player-play-button-37x37.png] Launch Video Print Cite This! Close [icon_close.gif] Please copy/paste the following text to properly cite this HowStuffWorks article: Freeman, Shanna. "How the Placebo Effect Works" 13 January 2009. HowStuffWorks.com. 03 January 2014. Cite Feedback Page 1 Introduction to How the Placebo Effect Works 2 Placebos: Is It All in Your Head? 3 Placebos in Research 4 Prescribing Placebos 5 The Placebo Prescribing Controversy 6 Lots More Information [hsw-article-keep-reading-long-button.png] [hsw-share-fb.png] [hsw-share-su.png] More To Explore All Things Science 10 Incredible Wind Power Facts 10 Incredible Wind Power Facts 10 Remarkable Exoplanets 10 Remarkable Exoplanets Fact or Fiction: Earthquakes Fact or Fiction: Earthquakes Top 10 Things That Women Invented Top 10 Things That Women Invented Strange, Spooky, Scary: The Supernatural Quiz Strange, Spooky, Scary: The Supernatural Quiz Watch 100 Greatest Discoveries videos » You Might Also Like [spacer.gif] Why do we remember pain? Scientists know that the brain's reward center teaches humans that certain behaviors lead to pleasure, but what about those that lead to pain? A clue lies in the fact that pain isn't just a physical sensation, but an emotional and psychological one as well. [spacer.gif] How the Placebo Effect Works When a kid scrapes his or her knee, a cute bandage is less about stanching the bleeding and more about psychological comfort. When a doctor gives an adult a placebo, is that any different? Popular Articles * Ultimate Pursuit of Happiness Quiz * Do we really get wiser with age? * Why do music and art move us? * Most Popular * Most Watched * Most Popular + [rightrail-numeral-1-36x36.png] Why do men have nipples? [men-nipples-100x60.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-2-36x36.png] 10 Completely False ‘Facts’ Everyone Knows [orig-10-false-facts-5-100x60.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-3-36x36.png] What will medicine consider unethical in 100 years? [medicine-consider-unethical-100x60.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-4-36x36.png] What if everyone on Earth spoke the same language? [speak-same-language-100x60.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-5-36x36.png] Top 5 Unsolved Brain Mysteries [unsolved-brain-mystery-100x60.jpg] * Most Watched + [rightrail-numeral-1-36x36.png] How to Make (Almost) Everything [46100.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-2-36x36.png] Understanding Bridges [29830.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-3-36x36.png] How do they do it? [46975.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-4-36x36.png] Why aren't the keys on my computer in alphabetical order? [41740.jpg] + [rightrail-numeral-5-36x36.png] The Most Important Things So Far, Seriously [27797.jpg] Don't Miss Brew Age: A Craft Beer Revolution [video-icon-blue-16x16.png] Brew Age: A Craft Beer Revolution Explore the Stuff of Genius behind incredible inventions. [video-icon-blue-16x16.png] Explore the Stuff of Genius behind incredible inventions. Look into the future with Fw:Thinking. [video-icon-blue-16x16.png] Look into the future with Fw:Thinking. CHECK OUT OUR PODCASTS [stuff-you-should-know-295x295.jpg] [brain-stuff-295x295b.jpg] [car-stuff-295x295.jpg] [how-to-stuff-295x295.jpg] [popstuff-150x150.jpg] [stuff-mom-never-295x295b.jpg] [stuff-dont-know-295x295.jpg] [stuff-to-blow-your-mind-295x295.jpg] [stuff-missed-history-class-295x295.jpg] [tech-stuff-295x295.jpg] [coolest-stuff-295x295.jpg] [stuff-genius-295x295.jpg] STUFF NATION NEWSLETTER THE HOW BEHIND TODAY'S NEWS ____________________ (BUTTON) SUBSCRIBE HOWSTUFFWORKS * Adventure * Auto * Culture * Entertainment * Health * Home & Garden * Lifestyle * Money * Science * Tech MORE STUFF * Blogs * Games * HSW Tools * RSS * Maps * Podcasts * Quizzes * Newsletters * Video OUR WEBSITES * Animal Planet * Animalist * Animal Planet Live * Destination America * Discovery Channel * Discovery Fit & Health * Discovery News * FWThinking * HSW Brazil * HSW China HowStuffWorks China * Investigation Discovery * Oprah Winfrey Network * Revision3 * Science Channel * Stuff You Should Know * Stuff to Blow Your Mind * Stuff of Genius * TLC * Discovery Education * Discovery Retreats * Store CUSTOMER SERVICE * Advertising * Contact Us * Help CORPORATE * About Us * Careers @ Discovery * Privacy Policy * Visitor Agreement TAKE US WITH YOU FOLLOW US © 1998-2014 HowStuffWorks, Inc IFRAME: __bkframe § * Browse + Browse Placebos + Browse Prescriptions * Tour * Blog * Login * Join (BUTTON) * Browse + Browse Placebos + Browse Prescriptions * Tour * Blog * Login * Join Take a Tour Join Placebo Effect Quitsmoking dark11 Weightloss light eating1 Purpose advice8 Selfimprovement action3 Fitness light5 Loneliness advice1 Weightloss advice16 Selfesteem advice5 Focus advice5 Exercise light4 Family humor5 * About Us * Contact Us * Blog * Terms of service * Privacy policy § * Facebook * Google Plus * Twitter * YouTube * Español * Asian & Pacific Languages * Sign Up for Email * Sign In * Register American Cancer Society Donate Join the fight against cancer Thanks to American Cancer Society volunteers, cancer patients receive thousands of free rides to treatment every year. * How can we help you? ____________________ (BUTTON) Search * Live Chat * 800-227-2345 * Home * Learn About Cancer * Stay Healthy * Find Support & Treatment * Explore Research * Get Involved * Find Local ACS Image+%2D+Hero%2D+Home+%2D+Mobile+%2D+20121022 Find Support & Treatment » Treatments and Side Effects » Treatment Types » Placebo Effect Share this Page Close Push escape to close share window. Print Share Save Saved this Article Close Push escape to close saved articles window. My Saved Articles » My ACS » Your Local Offices Close + -Text Size Placebo Effect What is a placebo? A placebo (pluh-see-bow) is a substance or other kind of treatment that looks just like a regular treatment or medicine, but it’s not. It’s actually an inactive “look-alike” treatment or substance. This means it’s not a medicine. The person getting a placebo does not know for sure that the treatment is not real. Sometimes the placebo is in the form of a “sugar pill,” but a placebo can also be an injection, a liquid, or even a procedure. It’s designed to look like a real treatment, but doesn’t directly affect the illness. What is the placebo effect? Even though they do not act on the disease, placebos seem to affect how people feel (this happens in up to 1 out of 3 patients). A change in a person’s symptoms as a result of getting a placebo is called the placebo effect. Usually the term “placebo effect” speaks to the helpful effects a placebo has in relieving symptoms. This effect usually lasts only a short time. It’s thought to have something to do with the body’s chemical ability to briefly relieve pain or certain other symptoms. But sometimes the effect goes the other way, and causes unpleasant symptoms or worse. These may include headaches, nervousness, nausea, or constipation, to name a few of the possible “side effects.” The unpleasant effects that happen after getting a placebo are sometimes called the nocebo effect. Together, these 2 types of outcomes are sometimes called expectation effects. This means that the person taking the placebo may experience something along the lines of what he or she expects to happen. If a person expects to feel better, that may happen. If the person believes that he or she is getting a strong medicine, the placebo may be thought to cause the side effects. The placebo does not cause any of these effects directly. Instead, the person’s belief in or experience of the placebo helps change the symptoms, or change the way the person perceives the symptoms. Some patients can have the placebo effect without getting a pill, shot, or procedure. Some may just feel better from visiting the doctor or doing something else they believe will help. That type of placebo effect seems most related to the degree of confidence and faith the patient has in the doctor or activity. How are placebos used? Placebos may be used in clinical trials. Clinical trials are research studies testing new drugs or other treatments in volunteers. Before a new treatment is used on people, it’s studied in the lab. If lab studies suggest the treatment will work, the next step is to test it on animals. If that also gives promising results, it may then be tested in clinical trials to see if it has value for patients. The main questions the researchers want to answer are: * Does this treatment work? * Does it work better than what we’re now using? * What side effects does it cause? * Do the benefits of the treatment outweigh the risks? * Which patients are most likely to find this treatment helpful? If standard treatments for the disease are already available, the new treatment is usually compared to one of these treatments. This tells researchers if the new treatment is as good as or better than the one that’s currently available. If there’s no approved treatment for an illness or condition, some people in the study may be given a placebo, while others get the new treatment being tested. The main reason to have a placebo group is to be sure that any effects that happen are actually caused by the treatment and not some other factor. The placebo looks, tastes, or feels just like the actual treatment, so that the patient’s and the doctor’s expectations don’t affect the outcomes. The placebo control makes it possible to “blind” patients and doctors to what treatment they’re getting. In a double-blind controlled study, neither the volunteers taking part in the study nor their doctors know who’s getting which treatment. This study design helps avoid biases in measuring outcomes that can be caused by the researchers’ or the patients’ expectations about the treatment. This is not as big a problem in studies of cancer treatment, where objective outcome measures are most often used. It’s more likely in studies that require patient reporting for symptoms like depression, sleeplessness, or pain. If you would like to know more about clinical trials, see our document called Clinical Trials: What You Need to Know. People in a study that uses a placebo should always know that there’s a chance they could be getting a placebo. It’s not OK to give someone a treatment in a clinical trial and not mention that it could be a placebo. Please see our document Informed Consent for more information on this. Those who get placebos in medical studies serve an important role. Their responses help provide a good way to measure the actual effect of the treatment being tested. The placebo group provides an important baseline with which to compare the treatment group. It helps researchers see what would have happened without the treatment, though both groups may still have some short-term effects based on what the patient expects. For instance, illnesses that sometimes go away on their own might be thought to get better because of the medicine, unless there’s a placebo group and those people get better too. On the other side, bad effects that were going to happen anyway, or that occur from some unrelated cause, may be blamed on the treatment unless they also happen to people in the placebo group. How does the placebo effect work? In the past, some researchers have questioned whether there’s convincing proof that the placebo effect is a real effect. But there are studies showing that the placebo effect is real. For example, scientists have recorded brain activity in response to placebo. Since many scientific tests have shown the placebo effect, it’s one way we know for sure that the mind and body are connected. Some scientific evidence suggests that the placebo effect may be partly due to the release of endorphins in the brain. Endorphins are the body’s natural pain killers. But there’s probably more to it than this. Many think the placebo effect occurs because the patient believes in the substance, the treatment, or the doctor. The patient’s thoughts and feelings somehow cause short-term physical changes in the brain or body. The patient expects to feel better, and so he or she does feel better for some time. But even if a person feels better after taking a placebo, it doesn’t mean the person’s illness or symptoms were not real. For instance, the person may feel less anxious, so stress hormones drop. Taking a placebo may change their perception — for example, a person might re-interpret a sharp pain as uncomfortable tingling. What’s commonly called the placebo effect even plays a role in mainstream medicine. Many people feel better after they get medical treatments that they expect to work. But the opposite can also happen, and this seems to support the idea of the expectation effect even more. For example, in one study, people with Alzheimer’s disease got less relief from pain medicines. These patients required higher doses — possibly because they had forgotten they were getting the drugs, or they forgot that the pain medicines had worked for them before. This suggests that past experiences also play into the placebo effect. In one study that looked at the placebo effect in pain relief, one group got a real pain medicine and the other did not. In the following days, both groups were given a placebo that looked like the real pain medicine. Those who had gotten the real pain medicine were able to tolerate more pain than those who had not gotten pain medicines before. In the same study, people who were given a drug that raised a certain hormone level beforehand actually had a similar (but smaller) hormone response when they were given a placebo later. Those who had not gotten the real drug beforehand had no change in their hormone levels when they got the placebo, even though they were told that they would. This helped to separate the power of the researcher telling them they would have an effect from the learned experience of having the effect in the past. This type of learned response after personal experience is called the conditioning effect. It seems to be part of what we call the placebo effect. The nocebo effect, in which a person has more symptoms or side effects after a placebo, is still being studied. Researchers believe it may be partly explained by a substance in the body that sends messages through the nerves. When a person is anxious, for instance, the substance is activated and the person feels more pain than a person who isn’t anxious. The nocebo effect shows up in the brain: brain-imaging studies have shown that pain is more intense when a person expects more pain than when they don’t. This is linked to changes in certain brain regions on the imaging studies. Although we may not know exactly how it works, the idea that the mind can affect the body has been around for thousands of years and is well-proven in certain situations. Many ancient cultures depended on mind-body connections to treat illness. Shamans or medicine men would not have viewed their efforts as placebos. But their healing powers may have worked partly through the patient’s strong belief that the shaman’s treatments would restore health. Or it could be that a sick person was going to get better anyway, but the recovery was thought to be because of the treatment — which might have really done nothing for the illness. Because placebos often have an effect, even if the effect doesn’t last long, some people think that the placebo produced a cure. But placebos do not cure. And in studies where doctors are looking at whether a tumor shrinks, placebos have very little, if any, effect. Still, placebos clearly can help reduce certain symptoms such as pain, anxiety, and trouble sleeping in some people. In earlier times, placebos were sometimes given by doctors out of frustration or desperation, because nothing else was available or seemed to work. They might even be tried today — a 2008 study found that nearly half of the doctors polled said that they used a placebo when they felt that it might help the patient feel better. Sometimes if the placebo looks more real, the person may think it’s an active medicine or treatment and believe in its power even more. For example, a larger pill may look more powerful than a small pill. And in some people, an injection may have a stronger placebo effect than a pill. Some believe that placebos seem to work because many illnesses improve over time even without treatment. People may also take better care of themselves by exercising, eating healthier, or resting if they are taking a placebo. Just as natural endorphins may relieve pain once they are released, some research shows the brain may respond to an imagined scene much as it would to something it actually sees. A placebo may help the brain remember a time before the symptoms and bring about a chemical change. This is a theory called remembered wellness. Some scientists believe that the effects of many alternative therapies may simply be a placebo effect. If the patient believes in the treatment and wants it to work, it can seem to do so, at least for a while. If this effect worked on an illness that usually would not get better on its own, and it lasted, it would be considered a real cure, not a placebo effect. Finally, there’s evidence to suggest that what a patient expects about real medicines can influence how the patient feels after the medicine is taken. Even though responses from real drugs aren’t typically thought of as placebo effects, some short-term effects are affected by expectations — good ones as well as bad. There’s still much to find out about the placebo effect and all the ways it may work. Researchers agree that the placebo effect cannot cure people or make them live substantially longer. Still, they continue to study it in order to learn more about it, and find out whether and how it may be used to help people feel better. Other things that can add to or be confused for the placebo effect Certain other factors can affect study outcomes, and may be confused with the placebo effect. These are effects that can cause a study to report that a placebo (or even a treatment) had some effect when in reality, it didn’t. Timing of unrelated events Along with the placebo or nocebo effect, incidental events (unrelated effects that might have happened without the placebo) might also be linked to the placebo because of their timing. For example, a headache or rash that happens soon after taking a placebo could be caused by something else entirely, but the person might think the placebo caused it. The same can be said for good outcomes: if a person happened to start feeling better after taking a placebo, that improvement may be thought to be due to the placebo. Healing or changes in symptoms A health problem that improves on its own (many do) can sometimes add to what’s thought of as the placebo effect. Even in serious conditions such as cancer, some types appear to get better and worse on their own, although they continue to spread and worsen over time. This is part of the effect of timing, noted above. A person who was taking a placebo when symptoms started improving on their own is very likely to believe that the placebo had some effect. And a self-limited illness that goes away completely on its own at such a time might have the placebo-taker convinced it was a miracle drug. Patient reporting Lack of blinding can affect reporting of these kinds of coincidental timing effects and belief in what caused them. For example, people who know they’re getting placebos will know that their headache is not due to the medicine, and may not report it. Those who know they are getting the real medicine are more likely to believe that the medicine is causing the headache, and are more likely to report it. Blinding helps to balance these incidental timing effects. Self-report of symptoms on a scale Studies comparing placebo to no treatment found that patients were more likely to tell the study investigator that there was some improvement in their comfort if they were on placebo. This was observed when the patient had to rank their symptoms on a scale, say, from 1 to 10. When the patients’ rankings were compared with outcomes others could measure (signs, like swelling or movement) there would be no improvement in the measurable outcome. Some researchers believe this is due to patients wanting to give the investigator good news. It may also be that the patient is comparing current symptoms to the worst discomfort this problem had caused in the past. Getting outside treatment The patient who isn’t getting better in a study may also seek outside medical help without mentioning it to investigators. Or outside treatment for a related problem may help the symptom being studied in the clinical trial. This can affect outcomes for both the placebo and treatment groups. Study dropouts People who are not getting better in the study, including those on placebo, are more likely to leave the study before it’s over. This means that people who aren’t helped are less likely to be counted in the final analysis. This makes any treatment look as if it works better than it does, because the people who felt better during treatment were more likely to have their results counted at the end of the study than those who didn’t. To learn more More information from your American Cancer Society Here is more information you might find helpful. You also can order free copies of our documents from our toll-free number, 1-800-227-2345, or read them on our Web site, www.cancer.org. Clinical Trials: What You Need to Know (also in Spanish) Complementary and Alternative Methods for Cancer Management (also in Spanish) Dietary Supplements: How to Know What Is Safe Guidelines for Using Complementary and Alternative Methods Learning About New Cancer Treatments Informed Consent (also in Spanish) American Cancer Society Operational Statement on Complementary and Alternative Methods of Cancer Management Along with the above, information on many different types of cancer treatment, including complementary and alternative treatments, are available at no cost to you from the American Cancer Society. You can find them on our Web site or request a copy from our toll-free number as noted above. Other organizations and Web sites* Today there’s a great deal of interest in complementary and alternative therapies. Mass communication, especially the Internet, makes it possible for people to share ideas and information very quickly. But too often information on the Internet is written by promoters of useless treatments. Along with the American Cancer Society, the following is a partial list of Web sites and phone numbers of reputable groups that provide information on complementary and alternative therapies, and in some cases, other cancer information: National Cancer Institute Web site: www.cancer.gov Toll-free number: 1-800-422-6237 (1-800-4-CANCER) TTY: 1-800-332-8615 Offers information about cancer, treatment, clinical trials, and related help on many cancer topics National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine (NCCAM) Web site: http://nccam.nih.gov Toll-free number: 1-888-644-6226 TTY: 1-866-464-3615 Offers more details on many complementary treatments Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center About Herbs and Botanicals Web site: www.mskcc.org/cancer-care/integrative-medicine/about-herbs-botanicals-o ther-products Has summaries of information on a number of complementary and alternative treatments MD Anderson Complementary/Integrative Medicine Education Resources (CIMER) Web site: www.mdanderson.org/education-and-research/resources-for-professionals/c linical-tools-and-resources/cimer/therapies/index.html United States Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Information Center Web site: http://fnic.nal.usda.gov Choose “Dietary Supplements” from the left menu bar for information on dietary supplements, including vitamins and minerals United States Food and Drug Administration Web site: www.fda.gov/Food/DietarySupplements/default.htm Toll-free number: 1-888-463-6332 (1-888-INFO-FDA) Has information on dietary and food supplements. You can also report side effects or other adverse events from a dietary supplement by calling Medwatch at 1-800-332-1088 (1-800-FDA-1088) National Council Against Health Fraud Web Site: www.ncahf.org To help you discern scammers and fraudsters from real cancer treatment Quackwatch Web Site: www.quackwatch.org Reports on so-called cancer treatments that don’t actually work *Inclusion on this list does not imply endorsement by the American Cancer Society No matter who you are, we can help. Contact us anytime, day or night, for information and support. Call us at 1-800-227-2345 or visit www.cancer.org. References Bausell RB. Snake Oil Science. 2007. New York: Oxford University Press. Benedetti F, Arduino C, Costa S, et al. Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain. 2006;121:133-144. Benedetti F, Pollo A, Lopiano L, et al. Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. J Neurosci. 2003;23(10):4315-4323. Beyerstein BL. Distinguishing science from pseudoscience. 1995. Victoria, B.C.: The Centre for Curriculum and Professional Development. Accessed at www.sfu.ca/~beyerste/research/articles/02SciencevsPseudoscience.pdf on November 8, 2007. Content no longer available. Boström H. Placebo—the forgotten drug. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1997;23 Suppl 3:53-57. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol. 2007;20(5):435-439. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. Pain. 2009 Mar 9. Haour, F. Mechanisms of the placebo effect and of conditioning. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2005;12:195-200. Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Is the placebo powerless? Systematic review with 52 new randomized trials comparing placebo with no treatment. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2005;257:394-396. Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. N Engl J Med. 2001;344(21):1594-1602. Kaptchuk TJ. The placebo effect in alternative medicine: Can the performance of a healing ritual have clinical significance? Ann Intern Med. 2002;136:817-825. Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, et al. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ. 2008;336(7651):999-1003. Link J, Haggard R, Kelly K, Forrer D. Placebo/nocebo symptom reporting in a sham herbal supplement trial. Eval Health Prof. 2006;29:394-406. Pollo A, Amanzio M, Arslanian A, et al. Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance. Pain. 2001;93(1):77-84. Wampold, BE, Minami T, Tierney SC , Baskin TW, Bhati, KS. The placebo is powerful: Estimating placebo effects in medicine and psychotherapy from randomized clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2005;61:835-854. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, et al. Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects. Neuron. 2007;55:325-336. Sherman R, Hickner J. Academic physicians use placebos in clinical practice and believe in the mind-body connection. J Gen Intern Med. 2008;23(1):7-10. Victoria (Australia) Better Health Channel. Placebo effect. Last reviewed October 2009. Accessed at www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au/bhcv2/bhcarticles.nsf/pages/Placebo_effect? OpenDocument on September 28, 2012. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, et al. Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303:1162-1167. Find Support & Treatment Topics * Understanding Your Diagnosis * Finding and Paying for Treatment * Treatments and Side Effects * Survivorship: During and After Treatment * Children and Cancer * Caregivers * Nearing the End of Life * Find Support Programs and Services in Your Area EOY_Mobile_footer EOY_Footer_donate * Cancer Information * Cancer Basics * Cancer Prevention & Detection * Signs & Symptoms of Cancer * Treatments & Side Effects * Cancer Facts & Statistics * News About Cancer * Expert Voices Blog * Programs & Services * Breast Cancer Support * TLC Hair Loss & Mastectomy Products * Hope Lodge® Lodging * Rides To Treatment * Online Support Communities * WhatNext * ACS Events * Making Strides Against Breast Cancer Walks * Coaches vs. Cancer * Cancer Prevention Study (CPS-3) * Relay For Life Events * College Relay For Life * Relay Recess * About ACS * About Us * Contact Us * Local Offices * Volunteer * Employment * Become a Supplier * Report Fraud or Abuse * More ACS Sites * Bookstore * ACS CAN * Gift Shop * Global Health * Finish the Fight * Press Room * Mobile Site * Help * Site Map * Privacy * Accessibility * Terms of Use * State Fundraising Notices * Site Comments * BBB Better Business Bureau * HON Health On The Net * National Health Council Standards of Excellence Certification Program National Health Council © 2014 American Cancer Society, Inc. All rights reserved. The American Cancer Society is a qualified 501(c)(3) tax-exempt organization. Cancer.org is provided courtesy of the Leo and Gloria Rosen family. § 403 Forbidden __________________________________________________________________ nginx/0.7.65 § Harvard Magazine Your independent source for Harvard news since 1898 | DONATE * Current Issue * Class Notes * Obituaries * Contact [ADTECH;loc=300;key=key1+key2+key3+key4;grp=[group]] Search this site: _______________ Search * News + Allston Campus + Capital Campaign + Graduate Schools + Harvard College + Harvard Finances + Online Education * Research + Humanities + Science + Social Sciences + International * Students * Alumni + Profiles + HAA News * Arts + Architecture + Books + Dance + Fashion + Film + Food + Literary Life + Museums and Collections + Music + Television + Theater + Visual Arts * Sports * Harvardiana * Opinion * Multimedia + Audio + Video + Photos + Puzzles * Classifieds + U.S. Vacation Rentals + Int'l Vacation Rentals + Real Estate for Sale + Real Estate for Rent + Personals + Products & Services + Destination: France + Camp & School Plus > * January-February 2014 * Back Issues * Harvard Finances * Undergraduate Fellows * Web App Features The Placebo Phenomenon An ingenious researcher finds the real ingredients of “fake” medicine. Research * Photograph by Jim HarrisonTed Kaptchuk in his home office in Cambridge Print | PDF | Reprints by Cara Feinberg January-February 2013 Email Keywords Harvard Medical School, health and medicine, placebos, Ted Kaptchuk Two weeks into Ted Kaptchuk’s first randomized clinical drug trial, nearly a third of his 270 subjects complained of awful side effects. All the patients had joined the study hoping to alleviate severe arm pain: carpal tunnel, tendinitis, chronic pain in the elbow, shoulder, wrist. In one part of the study, half the subjects received pain-reducing pills; the others were offered acupuncture treatments. And in both cases, people began to call in, saying they couldn’t get out of bed. The pills were making them sluggish, the needles caused swelling and redness; some patients’ pain ballooned to nightmarish levels. “The side effects were simply amazing,” Kaptchuk explains; curiously, they were exactly what patients had been warned their treatment might produce. But even more astounding, most of the other patients reported real relief, and those who received acupuncture felt even better than those on the anti-pain pill. These were exceptional findings: no one had ever proven that acupuncture worked better than painkillers. But Kaptchuk’s study didn’t prove it, either. The pills his team had given patients were actually made of cornstarch; the “acupuncture” needles were retractable shams that never pierced the skin. The study wasn’t aimed at comparing two treatments. It was designed to compare two fakes. Although Kaptchuk, an associate professor of medicine, has spent his career studying these mysterious human reactions, he doesn’t argue that you can simply “think yourself better.” “Sham treatment won’t shrink tumors or cure viruses,” he says. But researchers have found that placebo treatments—interventions with no active drug ingredients—can stimulate real physiological responses, from changes in heart rate and blood pressure to chemical activity in the brain, in cases involving pain, depression, anxiety, fatigue, and even some symptoms of Parkinson’s. The challenge now, says Kaptchuk, is to uncover the mechanisms behind these physiological responses—what is happening in our bodies, in our brains, in the method of placebo delivery (pill or needle, for example), even in the room where placebo treatments are administered (are the physical surroundings calming? is the doctor caring or curt?). The placebo effect is actually many effects woven together—some stronger than others—and that’s what Kaptchuk hopes his “pill versus needle” study shows. The experiment, among the first to tease apart the components of placebo response, shows that the methods of placebo administration are as important as the administration itself, he explains. It’s valuable insight for any caregiver: patients’ perceptions matter, and the ways physicians frame perceptions can have significant effects on their patients’ health. For the last 15 years, Kaptchuk and fellow researchers have been dissecting placebo interventions—treatments that, prior to the 1990s, had been studied largely as foils to “real” drugs. To prove amedicine is effective, pharmaceutical companies must show not only that their drug has the desired effects, but that the effects are significantly greater than those of a placebo control group. Both groups often show healing results, Kaptchuk explains, yet for years, “We were struggling to increase drug effects while no one was trying to increase the placebo effect.” Last year, he and colleagues from several Harvard-affiliated hospitals created the Program in Placebo Studies and the Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS), headquartered at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center—the only multidisciplinary institute dedicated solely to placebo study. It’s a nod to changing attitudes in Western medicine, and a direct result of the small but growing group of researchers like Kaptchuk who study not if, but how, placebo effects work. Explanations for the phenomenon come from fields across the scientific map—clinical science, psychology, anthropology, biology, social economics, neuroscience. Disregarding the knowledge that placebo treatments can affect certain ailments, Kaptchuk says, “is like ignoring a huge chunk of healthcare.” As caregivers, “we should be using every tool in the box.” Western medicine, however, has been slow to agree with him—partly because of his message, and in his case, often because of the messenger. An acupuncturist by training, he is an unlikely leader in the halls of academia. With a degree in Chinese medicine from an institute in Macao, Kaptchuk is one of the few faculty members at Harvard Medical School (HMS) with neither a Ph.D. nor M.D.—“a debit, not a credit at most medical schools,” says Finland professor of clinical pharmacology emeritus Peter Goldman, one of his early Harvard advisers. (Kaptchuk’s diploma is recognized as a doctorate in many states, but not in Massachusetts.) When Kaptchuk came to Harvard in 1995, “he knew about Chinese herbs and healing needles, and he’d written a very fine book on Chinese medicine [The Web That Has No Weaver (1983)],” says Goldman, “but he didn’t know the first thing about how to conduct clinical studies.” Kaptchuk joined the faculty as an instructor in medicine and apprenticed himself to several seasoned clinicians and investigators. Within a few years, he was winning National Institutes of Health grants and publishing in medicine’s top journals. “What his colleagues saw was a fierce intellect and curiosity,” said Goldman. “He was asking questions no one was asking.” Ironically, says Kaptchuk, it was his success as an acupuncturist that made him leave the profession for academia. “Patients who came to me got better,” he says, but sometimes their relief began even before he’d started his treatments. He didn’t doubt the value of acupuncture, but he suspected something else was at work. His hunch was that it was his engagement with patients—and perhaps even the act of caring itself. For his ideas to gain traction with Western doctors, however, Kaptchuk knew he needed scientific proof. His chance would come in the early 2000s in a collaboration with gastroenterologists studying irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), a chronic gastrointestinal disorder accompanied by pain and constipation. The experiment split 262 adults with IBS into three groups: a no-treatment control group, told they were on a waiting list for treatment; a second group who received sham acupuncture without much interaction with the practitioner; and a third group who received sham acupuncture with great attention lavished upon them—at least 20 minutes of what Kaptchuk describes as “very schmaltzy” care (“I’m so glad to meet you”; “I know how difficult this is for you”; “This treatment has excellent results”). Practitioners were also required to touch the hands or shoulders of members of the third group and spend at least 20 seconds lost in thoughtful silence. The results were not surprising: the patients who experienced the greatest relief were those who received the most care. But in an age of rushed doctor’s visits and packed waiting rooms, it was the first study to show a “dose-dependent response” for a placebo: the more care people got—even if it was fake—the better they tended to fare. Kaptchuk’s innovative studies were among the first to separate components of the placebo effect, explains Applebaum professor of medicine Russell Phillips, director of the Center for Primary Care at HMS. For years, doctor-patient interactions were lumped into a generic “placebo response”: a sum of such variables as patients’ reporting bias (a conscious or unconscious desire to please the researchers); patients simply responding to doctors’ attention; the different methods of placebo delivery; and symptoms subsiding without treatment—the inevitable trajectory of most chronic ailments. “There was simply no way to quantify the ritual of medicine,” says Phillips of the doctor-patient interaction. And the ritual, he adds, is the one finding from placebo research that doctors can apply to their practice immediately. But other placebo treatments (sham acupuncture, pills, or other fake interventions) are nowhere near ready for clinical application—and Kaptchuk is not recommending that they should be. Such treatments all require deception on the part of doctors, an aspect of placebo medicine that raises serious ethical questions for practitioners. This was disturbing for Kaptchuk, too; deception played no role in his own success as a healer. But years of considering the question led him to his next clinical experiment: What if he simply told people they were taking placebos? The question ultimately inspired a pilot study, published by the peer-reviewed science and medicine journal PLOS ONE in 2010, that yielded his most famous findings to date. His team again compared two groups of IBS sufferers. One group received no treatment. The other patients were told they’d be taking fake, inert drugs (delivered in bottles labeled “placebo pills”) and told also that placebos often have healing effects. The study’s results shocked the investigators themselves: even patients who knew they were taking placebos described real improvement, reporting twice as much symptom relief as the no-treatment group. That’s a difference so significant, says Kaptchuk, it’s comparable to the improvement seen in trials for the best real IBS drugs. Although this IBS “open-label” study was small and has yet to be replicated, fellow placebo researcher Frank Miller of the department of bioethics at the National Institutes of Health considers it a significant step toward legitimizing placebo studies. But to really change minds in mainstream medicine, Miller says, researchers have to show biological evidence that minds actually change—a feat achieved only in the last decade through imaging technology such as positron emission tomography (PET) scans and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The first evidence of a physiological basis for the placebo effect appeared in the late 1970s, when researchers studying dental patients found that by chemically blocking the release of endorphins—the brain’s natural pain relievers—scientists could also block the placebo effect. This suggested that placebo treatments spurred chemical responses in the brain that are similar to those of active drugs, a theory borne out two decades later by brain-scan technology. Researchers like neuroscientist Fabrizio Benedetti at the University of Turin have since shown that many neurotransmitters are at work—including chemicals that use the same pathways as opium and marijuana. Studies by other researchers have shown that placebos increase dopamine (a chemical that affects emotions and sensations of pleasure and reward) in the brains of Parkinson’s patients, and patients suffering from depression who’ve been given placebos reveal changes in electrical and metabolic activity in several different regions of the brain. Kaptchuk’s team has investigated the neural mechanisms of placebos in collaboration with the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at Massachusetts General Hospital. In two fMRI studies published in the Journal of Neuroscience in 2006 and 2008, they showed that placebo treatments affect the areas of the brain that modulate pain reception, as do negative side effects from placebo treatment—“nocebo effects.” (Nocebo is Latin for “I shall harm”; placebo means “I shall please.”) But nocebo effects also activate the hippocampus, a different area associated with memory and anxiety. As happened with Kaptchuk’s patients in the “pill versus needle” study, the headaches, nausea, insomnia, and fatigue that result from fake treatments can be painfully real, afflicting about a quarter of those assigned to placebo treatment in drug trials(see “The Nocebo Effect,” May-June 2005). “What we ‘placebo neuroscientists’…have learned [is] that therapeutic rituals move a lot of molecules in the patients’ brain, and these molecules are the very same as those activated by the drugs we give in routine clinical practice,” Benedetti wrote in an e-mail. “In other words, rituals and drugs use the very same biochemical pathways to influence the patient’s brain.” It’s those advances in “hard science,” he added, that have given placebo research a legitimacy it never enjoyed before. This new visibility has encouraged not only research funds but also interest from healthcare organizations and pharmaceutical companies. As healthcare companies increasingly reward doctors for maintaining patients’ health (rather than for the number of procedures they perform), “research like Ted’s becomes increasingly relevant,” says Minot professor of medicine and HMS dean for graduate education David Golan, a professor of biological chemistry and molecular pharmacology. This year, the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, the nation’s largest philanthropy focused on health and healthcare, awarded Kaptchuk’s PiPS program a $250,000 grant to support a series of seminars at Harvard designed to connect placebo experts with researchers in related fields. And the latest findings to emerge from PiPS—a 2012 study showing that genetic variations may explain why only certain people respond to placebo effects—has caught the attention of the Food and Drug Administration. That study, published last Octoberin PLOS ONE, showed that patients with a certain variation of a gene linked to the release of dopamine were more likely to respond to sham acupuncture than patients with a different variation—findings that could change the way pharmaceutical companies conduct drug trials, says Gunther Winkler, principal of ASPB Consulting, LLC, which advises biotech and pharmaceutical firms. Companies spend millions of dollars and often decades testing drugs; every drug must outperform placebos if it is to be marketed. “If we can identify people who have a low predisposition for placebo response, drug companies can preselect for them,” says Winkler. “This could seriously reduce the size, cost, and duration of clinical trials…bringing cheaper drugs to the market years earlier than before.” Not all of Kaptchuk’s studies have been so warmly received. Though few academics quarrel with the quality of his research, he’s remained a prime target for such watchdog groups as Quackwatch and The Skeptics’ Society, organizations that question nonconventional medical approaches. (Other well-known targets include Deepak Chopra, Andrew Weil ’63, M.D. ’68, and the late Nobel Prize winner Linus Pauling.) In 2011, he and a team of researchers published a paper in The New England Journal of Medicine (NEJM) that raised the hackles of some of his fiercest critics. That paper (praised by scholars as one of the most carefully controlled and definitive placebo studies ever done) described a study of 40 asthma patients given four different interventions: active treatments with real albuterol inhalers; placebo treatments with fake inhalers that delivered no medication; sham acupuncture treatments; and intervals with no treatment at all. The patients returned for 12 sequential visits, receiving each type of treatment three times—a novel approach in placebo study that created a large amount of data (480 treatments in total) and turned subjects into their own controls (if patients are compared to themselves from one treatment to the next, researchers can eliminate subjects’ individual differences as a variable). The researchers had hoped to find improved lung function with both the real and sham treatments; what they found instead was that only the real treatment yielded results—the others showed no significant improvement. Yet when Kaptchuk’s team measured patients’ own assessments of improvement, the researchers found no difference reported between the real and sham treatments: the patients’subjective responses directly contradicted their own objective physical measures. To Dr. Harriet Hall, a retired family physician who writes critically about alternative and complementary medicine for such publications as Skeptic Magazine and Skeptical Inquirer, this discrepancy between objective and subjective results is precisely where the danger lies. As she told a reporter for The Atlantic in December 2011, following the publication of Kaptchuk’s NEJM study, “Asthma can be fatal. If the patient’s lung function is getting worse but a placebo makes them feel better, they might delay treatment until it is too late.” To Kaptchuk’s team, on the other hand, the conflicting results not only reveal important lessons for researchers and clinicians, but illuminate a gap that is central to placebo research. “Placebos have limitations, and we need to know what they are,” Kaptchuk says. “We’d hoped for measurable objective changes in breathing; what we got instead was a more precise diagram of placebo effects and how clearly the ritual of medicine makes people more comfortable.” That in itself is important information, he says. “Our job is to make people feel better,” and though this study was small, “what we’ve really done here is open up a new set of questions.” No one has yet studied how long-term experience with the ritual of medicine might ultimately affect the course of chronic afflictions, he says. “We hope we’ve opened up that path.” Kaptchuk and his team have begun to take steps in that direction, continuing to ask new questions and push the boundaries of placebo research. A study published online this past year in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences demonstrated that the placebo response can occur even at the unconscious level. The team showed that images flashed on a screen for a fraction of a second—too quickly for conscious recognition—could trigger the response,but only if patients had learned earlier to associate those specific images with healing. Thus, when patients enter a room containing medical equipment they associate with the possibility of feeling better, “the mind may automatically make associations that lead to actual positive health outcomes,” says psychiatry research fellow Karin Jensen, the study’s lead author. Those findings led to the team’s most recent work: imaging the brains of physicians whilethey treat patients—a side of the treatment equation that no one had previously examined. (The researchers constructed an elaborate set-up in which the doctors lay in fMRI machines specially equipped to enable them both to see their patients outside the machine and administer what they thought was a nerve-stimulating treatment.) “Doctors give subtle cues to their patients that neither may be aware of,” Kaptchuk explains. “They are a key ingredient in the ritual of medicine.” The hope is that the new brain scans will reveal how doctors’ unconscious thought figures into the treatment recipe. Within academia, Kaptchuk and his fellow researchers have not escaped criticism, but the voices have been few and far between. The most notable appeared in 2001 in the NEJM—the same publication that included Kaptchuk’s asthma study a decade later. In a paper titled, “Is the Placebo Powerless?” two Danish researchers reviewed 114 published studies involving 7,500 patients and questioned both the research methods and the short duration of most placebo studies. Many of the trials reviewed lacked “no-treatment” groups—an important control group missing even in Kaptchuk’s first “pill versus needle” study. But Kaptchuk’s response to such criticism is perhaps as rare in academia as his pedigree. “If I remember correctly,” said Asbjorn Hrobjartsson, the lead author of that 2001 paper during a recent phone conversation, “Ted was already thinking along the same lines as we were and realized [our paper] pointed out real methodological problems.” When Hrobjartsson came to speak at Harvard a year later, he stayed at Kaptchuk’s home, and in 2011, the two coauthored a paper (with the NIH’s Frank Miller) on biases and best practices in placebo study. When Kaptchuk talks about Hrobjartsson’s 2001 paper now, he winces, then nods with acceptance. “At first when I read it, I worried I’d be out of a job,” he says. “But frankly, [Hrobjartsson] was absolutely right.” In order to legitimize his findings to mainstream practitioners, the results must be expertly quantified, he acknowledges. “We have to transform the art of medicine into the science of care.” Cara Feinberg is a journalist working in print and documentary television. She can be reached through her website at www.CaraFeinberg.com. View the discussion thread. Most Read — Research 1. The Power of Patience 2. Made in the U.S.A. 3. Is There an App for That? 4. Popular Science 5. An Exercise Pill for the Brain? more > eNewsletter block Harvard News Stay informed. ____________________ Subscribe Learn more about Harvard Magazine’s free e-mail newsletters. Special Advertising Section: Shop The Square Special Advertising Section Harvard Square Shopping Guide Check out special offers from local Harvard Square Business Association members exclusively available to the readers of Harvard Magazine. Stay Connected Harvard Magazine Facebook Harvard Magazine Twitter Harvard Magazine Youtube Channel Harvard Magazine email The Current Issue of Harvard Magazine Current Issue Back Issues Back Issues More from Harvard Magazine * Max Sandvoss with some of his goats at the First Light Farm and Creamery in East Bethany, New York A Fever for Chèvre The brothers Sandvoss moved from glitter to goat cheese. * Porter University Professor Helen Vendler in Houghton Library with a copy of the Arion Press edition of John Ashbery's "Self-Portrait in a Convex Mirror" A Nearly Perfect Book The poetry critic, the publisher, and the art of bookmaking in a digital era * Destination: France Special travel section * Joshua Redman Saxophonically Speaking Joshua Redman’s sonorous voice on tenor * Rise of the Little Guy The Internet has led to cultural and economic changes that tilt power away from large organizations, argues Nicco Mele in a new book. * Camp and School Your guide to camps and summer programs for your children. * At her gallery, Emma Katz promotes sophisticated original work by young artists such as sculptor/photographer David Rodriguez, represented here (left to right) by A Love Song Played at a Very Specified Frequency, Holding Device, and The Thing About Distance. An Artful Business Where inspired artists meet aspiring collectors * Antinuclear activist Albert Bigelow (second from left) with crew members William Huntington ’28, Orion Sherwood, and George Willoughby in Hawaii in 1958 Vita: Albert Bigelow Brief life of an ardent pacifist: 1906-1993 * Buffering the Sun David Keith and the question of climate engineering News * Allston Campus * Harvard College * Graduate Schools * Harvard Finances Research * Humanities * Science * Social Sciences * International Students * Undergraduate Fellows Alumni * Profiles * HAA News Arts * Architecture * Books * Dance * Music * Visual Arts * Film * Theater * Literary Life * Fashion Sports * Football Harvardiana Opinion Multimedia * Audio * Video * Photos * Puzzles Advertise * Media Kit Classifieds * Classifieds FAQs * Rates * Place Your Ad * View Our Classifieds Special Advertising Sections * NE Real Estate Guide * NE Travel Guide * NE Dining Guide * NE Holiday Gift Guide * NE Spring Gift Guide * Shop Harvard Square Magazine * Current Issue * Back Issues Class Notes * Class Notes * Obituaries * Submit a Note * Submit an Obituary About Harvard Magazine * Contact Us * Donate * Subscribe * Reader Services * Email Newsletters * Web App FAQ Harvard Magazine * About Us | Privacy Policy | Report a Problem Readers should direct editorial feedback to letters[at]harvardmag [dot] com All contents ©1996-2013 Harvard Magazine Inc. All rights reserved. Find us on Google+ §  REFRESH(900 sec): http://www.medterms.com/script/main/art.asp?articlekey=31481 #Daily Health and Medical News Weekly Health and Medical News Digest Health and Medical Slideshows MedicineNet ___________________________________ Submit January 3, 2014 * Like Us * Follow Us * About Us * Newsletter * Home * Slideshow Pictures * Image Collection * Quizzes * Diseases & Conditions * Symptom Checker * Health & Living * Medications * MedTerms Dictionary * Pet Health Slideshow Pictures * All Slideshows * Newest Slideshows * Pet Health Slideshows Featured Slideshow Sexual Health Surprising Health Benefits of Sex View Slideshow » Image Collection (categories) 1. Allergic Skin Disorders 2. Bacterial Skin Diseases 3. Bites and Infestations 4. Diseases of Pigment 5. Fungal Skin Diseases 6. Medical Anatomy and Illustrations 7. Noncancerous, Precancerous & Cancerous Tumors 8. Oral Health Conditions 9. Papules, Scales, Plaques and Eruptions 10. Scalp, Hair and Nails 11. Sexually Transmitted Diseases (STDs) 12. Vascular, Lymphatic and Systemic Conditions 13. Viral Skin Diseases 14. Additional Skin Conditions Featured Image Picture of Psoriasis A reddish, scaly rash often located over the surfaces of the elbows, knees, scalp, and around or in the ears, navel, genitals or buttocks... View Image Gallery » See All Quizzes (119) 1. Happiness - Test your emotional IQ 2. Superfoods - Are you eating enough? 3. Fat and Fats - What do you know about fat? 4. Skin Pictures - Can you identify these conditions? 5. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome – What do you know about CFS? Featured Quiz Take the Sex & Love Quiz! The brain. The body. The bedroom. What do you know? View Quiz » Topic Centers * Conditions A-Z * Procedures A-Z * Allergies * Alzheimer's * Arthritis * Asthma * Blood Pressure * Cancer * Cholesterol * Chronic Pain * Cold & Flu * Depression * Diabetes * Digestion * Eyesight * Health & Living * Healthy Kids * Hearing & Ear * Heart * HIV/AIDS * Infectious Disease * Lung Conditions * Menopause * Men's Health * Mental Health * Migraine * Neurology * Oral Health * Pregnancy * Senior Health * Sexual Health * Skin * Sleep * Thyroid * Travel Health * Women's Health Health & Living * Diet & Weight Management * Exercise & Fitness * Nutrition, Food & Recipes * Prevention & Wellness Featured Topic Weight Loss The No-Diet Approach Learn More » Medications Supplements and Vitamins home > medterms medical dictionary a-z list > placebo effect definition * smaller * medium * larger Definition of Placebo effect Bad Bugs and Their Bites Slideshow Pictures 1. Bad Bugs and Their Bites Slideshow 2. Top Concentration Killers Slideshow 3. Understanding Multiple Sclerosis Slideshow Placebo effect: Also called the placebo response. A remarkable phenomenon in which a placebo -- a fake treatment, an inactive substance like sugar, distilled water, or saline solution -- can sometimes improve a patient's condition simply because the person has the expectation that it will be helpful. Expectation to plays a potent role in the placebo effect. The more a person believes they are going to benefit from a treatment, the more likely it is that they will experience a benefit. To separate out this power of positive thinking and some other variables from a drug's true medical benefits, companies seeking governmental approval of a new treatment often use placebo-controlled drug studies. If patients on the new drug fare significantly better than those taking placebo, the study helps support the conclusion that the medicine is effective. The power of positive thinking is not a new subject. The Talmud, the ancient compendium of rabbinical thought, states that: "Where there is hope, there is life." And hope is positive expectation, by another name. The scientific study of the placebo effect is usually dated to the pioneering paper published in 1955 on "The Powerful Placebo" by the anesthesiologist Henry K. Beecher (1904-1976). Beecher concluded that, across the 26 studies he analyzed, an average of 32% of patients responded to placebo. It has been shown that placebos have measurable physiological effects. They tend to speed up pulse rate, increase blood pressure, and improve reaction speeds, for example, when participants are told they have taken a stimulant. Placebos have the opposite physiological effects when participants are told they have taken a sleep-producing drug. The placebo effect is part of the human potential to react positively to a healer. A patient's distress may be relieved by something for which there is no medical basis. A familiar example is Band-Aid put on a child. It can make the child feel better by its soothing effect, though there is no medical reason it should make the child feel better. People who receive a placebo may also experience negative effects. They are like side effects with a medication and may include, for example, nausea, diarrhea and constipation. A negative placebo effect has been called the nocebo effect. Last Editorial Review: 6/14/2012 Search MedTerms: __________________________________________ Search Back to MedTerms online medical dictionary A-Z List Pill Identifier Tool Need help identifying pills and medications? Use the pill finder tool on RxList. Share this Article: Facebook Twitter Email Print Stay Informed! Get the latest health and medical information delivered direct to your inbox FREE! IFRAME: /script/main/nl-et-module-multiple-sm.asp Definition of Placebo effect Related Articles * Constipation * Diarrhea * Sleep MedicineNet Authored Webster's New World Medical Dictionary Learn more MedTerms Medical Word of the Day XML Adult Skin Problems Slideshow OnHealth Exclusives * Travel: Avoiding Bedbugs * LBP Home Remedies * Heartburn Home Remedies * Kidney Stones Symptoms * Ways to Treat Shingles * Yeast Infection Treatment * Pancreatitis Causes RxList [rxlist-pillimage.jpg] Pill Identifier Quick and easy pill identification Use Pill Finder Find a Pharmacy Includes 24 hour locations! Find it Now Drug Interaction Checker See potential drug interactions Check Interactions [featured-slideshow-psoriasis1.jpg] Adult Skin Problems Slideshow From WebMD Featured Centers * Itchy Rash? Could it Be Psoriasis? * The Dangers of DVT * 13 Things to Do Before Your Due Date Health Solutions From Our Sponsors * Probiotics IFRAME: http://www.medicinenet.com/htmlchunks/slideshow/slidermodule/rr_image_c oll_slider_mednet_green.htm STAY INFORMED Get the Latest health and medical information delivered direct to your inbox! IFRAME: /script/main/nl-et-module-multiple-rr.asp Symptom Checker: Your Guide to Symptoms & Signs: Pinpoint Your Pain IFRAME: http://www.medicinenet.com/htmlchunks/slideshow/slidermodule/bottom_sli der/article/mednet_slider.htm Health Solutions From Our Sponsors * Probiotics * Testosterone Treatment * High Triglycerides? * Rheumatoid Arthritis? * What are Probiotics? * Low T Medication * Help for Low T * Learn About Triglycerides * Erectile Dysfunction? * Monitoring RA Symptoms * Stages of Alzheimer's * Diabetes Help Center * Bent Fingers? * Low Testosterone? * Manage Triglycerides Health Categories: * Slideshow Pictures * Diseases & Conditions * Symptoms & Signs * Procedures & Tests * Medications * Healthy Living * Vitamins & Supplements * Image Collection * Quizzes * MedTerms Dictionary * Pet Health Popular Health Centers: * Allergies * Arthritis * Blood Pressure * Cancer * Chronic Pain * Cold & Flu * Depression * Diabetes * Digestion * Health & Living * Healthy Kids * Hearing & Ear * Heart * HIV/AIDS * Infectious Disease * Men's Health * Mental Health * Neurology * Pregnancy * Sexual Health * Skin * Thyroid * Women's Health * More... MedicineNet: * Privacy Policy * Contact Us * Site Map * WebMD Corporate * WebMD * Medscape * Medscape Reference * eMedicineHealth * RxList * OnHealth * BootsWebMD * Medscape France * Medscape Germany Follow Us On: * Facebook * Twitter * Google+ * RSS Ad Choices TRUSTe online privacy certification This website is certified by Health On the Net Foundation. Click to verify. ©1996-2014 MedicineNet, Inc. All rights reserved.Terms of Use. MedicineNet does not provide medical advice, diagnosis or treatment.See additional information. IFRAME: http://d.turn.com/r/dd/id/L21rdC83NC9jaWQvMTkyNDA3NTkvdC8x/cat/9 § #Edit RationalWiki (en) copyright RationalWiki Atom feed Bronze-level article Placebo effect From RationalWiki Jump to: navigation, search Tell me about your mother Psychology Icon psychology.svg Let us examine... * Cognitive biases * Mental health * Superstition * Famed psychologists Random examples * Cognitive revolution * Dream interpretation * Human Potential Movement * Natural horsemanship * Neurolinguistic programming * Persecution complex * Personality cult * Rationalism v - t - e “”No one doubts—certainly not I—that the mind exercises a powerful influence over the body. From the beginning of time, the sorcerer, the interpreter of dreams, the fortune-teller, the charlatan, the quack, the wild medicine-man, the educated physician, the mesmerist, and the hypnotist have made use of the client's imagination to help them in their work. They have all recognized the potency and availability of that force. Physicians cure many patients with a bread pill; they know that where the disease is only a fancy, the patient's confidence in the doctor will make the bread pill effective. —Mark Twain, Christian Science, 1907. The placebo effect is a psychosomatic phenomenon in which symptoms of a disease or condition lessen — or even appear to be cured completely — from the patient being merely exposed to a treatment. Believing that their condition will be improved, they will begin to feel better and perhaps identifiable symptoms may disappear, irrespective of whether the treatment has any chemical or pharmacological effect. Because of this, controlling for the placebo effect is an essential part of medicine. For example, sugar pills or saline solutions, which have no pharmacological properties, are commonly prescribed as placebo treatments to control for the effect the simple act of intervening in a patient's illness may have. Most alternative medicine is suggested to work through this method^[1] — mostly because any treatment that doesn't work through this method is labelled "medicine". Under placebo control, alternative medicines do not perform significantly better. Contents * 1 Causes + 1.1 Statistical vs individual experience + 1.2 Expectation effects * 2 Ethical considerations * 3 Use in medical trials * 4 The strangeness of the placebo effect * 5 Nocebo effect * 6 False placebo * 7 See also * 8 Footnotes [edit] Causes [edit] Statistical vs individual experience The placebo effect is quite a complex phenomenon. It is influenced by a multitude of things, including positive thought, reduced stress, the intensity or "drama" of the medical intervention, a patient's expectations of what the treatment can do, and the wider cultural meaning of medical treatment. A particularly interesting observation is how much these factors can be influenced subconsciously; a doctor merely telling someone a treatment will work may not produce such a dramatic effect if the doctor doesn't actually believe it too. Because of the complexity of issues surrounding what causes a sick patient to get "better", it's difficult to say if an individual was cured by a placebo effect, real treatment, regression to the mean or a complicated combination of all three. So, the placebo effect is best described as a statistical phenomenon, where prognosis improves for a certain percentage of patients simply exposed to a treatment of some kind. However, as the placebo effect is powered by belief in a treatment and exposure to it it can manifest in individuals to a certain extent. A homeopathic prescription of water containing a minute dilution of powdered oyster shell may bring some improvement to a patient who has total faith in the homeopathic method. A patient who is skeptical of homeopathy may experience a reduced effect or no effect at all, as demonstrated by James Randi when he regularly consumes massive overdoses of homeopathic sleeping pills on stage. If these two hypothetical patients begin to feel better anyway, the former would be likely to attribute it to the treatment while the latter would be likely to dismiss it as just feeling better anyway. This leads to selective reporting, and is why anecdotal evidence shouldn't be trusted over statistical data. [edit] Expectation effects Expectations of what drugs and medical treatments can, and do, change over time. Often the efficacy of real drugs decreases when a new version is released, as people expect the new one to be better. New treatments, which are perceived to be more effective because they exploit new technologies induce a placebo effect in addition to their real effect and, sometimes, this can lead to an over-estimate of their real efficacy over an existing treatment. This is why in controlled studies of new treatments, they are compared not with inactive placebos but the best effective treatment available (and not least because it is unethical to use placebopathy where a working treatment is known) and the experience between patients is minimised so that they genuinely cannot tell the difference between treatment A and treatment B. It is an observed phenomenon that placebo drugs distributed in flashier packaging tend to produce a stronger reaction than their plain packaging counterparts.^[2] There is also evidence that the placebo effect can work even if the person knows that it is a placebo, as observed in a study of patients with irritable bowel syndrome.^[3] However, the statement issued to patients read "placebo pills made of an inert substance, like sugar pills, that have been shown in clinical studies to produce significant improvement in IBS symptoms through mind-body self-healing processes" - which still creates an expectation of treatment despite being told there was no chemical reason that such treatment would help. In short, the placebo effect works if you expect the placebo effect to work. [edit] Ethical considerations There are a number of ethical issues surrounding the wilful use of placebos as a treatment - known as placebopathy. Many casual complaints can be dealt with by inactive placebo treatments (involving a sugar pill and a reassuring visit to a doctor) and this would save a significant amount of time, money and lowers the risk of drug dependency and overdose. Yet, despite these potential benefits to willingly issuing placebos, it is not considered ethical in modern medicine. Firstly, placebopathy involves medical professions deceiving their patients, which is considered bad ethical practice, and secondly there is a risk in delaying the treatment of a real condition due to a doctor mistrusting or underestimating their patient's symptoms. If a patient complaining of a headache is given a sugar pill to placate it, and it turns out to be a serious condition that requires treatment then at the very least this would be a serious law suit waiting to happen. Conversely, some medications that have been prescribed have later turned out to be no more effective than inactive placebos, yet can be an effective treatment for psychosomatic conditions or short-term illnesses. Because any treatment tends to produce a better effect than no treatment at all (hence the need for placebo control in trials, see below) it could be considered unethical to stop this practice of effective placebopathy. [edit] Use in medical trials Oscillococcinum, a well-known homeopathic remedy that "works" by the placebo effect. Because the placebo effect is so powerful, efficacy claims for medical treatments must be tested in an experiment that controls for this effect. This is usually achieved by running a double-blind test, where some patients receive the treatment being examined, and others get a placebo. Neither the patients or the administrators of the treatment know who is in which group. In order for the treatment to be proved useful, it must produce results that are better than the placebo at a statistically significant level. In practice the "placebo" is usually an existing treatment, as being better than an placebo isn't enough - a new treatment has to be better than the existing one! For example, in trials of keyhole surgery over conventional open heart surgery, elaborate schemes were undertaken to make patients all feel as if they had the same treatment - but all were treated for their condition, none received a full "fake" treatment that did nothing. Similarly, new pills would be compared with the existing treatments they aim to replace, not necessarily sugar pills or more "powerful" (but more restricted) drugs. Placebo treatments can often be quite difficult to design, but are still possible. As mentioned above, placebo surgeries have been performed using quite elaborate theatre, and often the precise wording that a doctor must use to describe a treatment to a patient is strictly controlled. Such treatments can be used to test not just efficacy, but the explanations offered by alternative medicine practitioners. A placebo treatment to test acupuncture, for example, could be designed to test claims about meridian lines would involve sticking the needles into a patient but in more randomised areas rather than the specific places the technique claims are "special". A second placebo treatment could test the effect of needles by changing the depth which they penetrate into the skin - acupuncture needles often go deeper than you think, while placebo needles penetrate only the top layer of the skin. Thirdly the effect of a visit to a practitioner can be controlled for by using entirely "sham" needles which prick the skin, causing indistinguishable pain sensations, but otherwise don't penetrate or cause any lasting effect. These three methods combine to test three separate areas whereby an alternative medicine can work. Quacks who push woo treatments rely on the placebo effect when arguing that their "cures" have an effect. This is exactly why medical tests need to pit a drug against a placebo: anything will usually perform better than nothing. Often, proponents of alternative medicine will reject placebo controlled experiments for any number of excuses. For example, many homopathic practitioners assert that treatments must be assigned individually and so are unsuited to the format of a randomised controlled trial. Yet, such things can be overcome by simply exposing the patient to everything about the treatment, including this individual treatment "design", and simply swapping the medication at the last moment, unknown to both the patient and practitioner. This sort of assertion is common in alternative medicine, but is obviously at odds with the mass-produced remedies that are sold to the public. Such trials have been done (very rarely does the alternative medicine world foot the bill for these), and they invariably come up negative. [edit] The strangeness of the placebo effect The power of the placebo effect produces many strange and interesting quirks. These indicate that the placebo effect may well be more than mere "mind over matter", and stem from what is known as a complex intervention. This is where any given treatment consists of not just the medicine issued, but the bedside manner of the doctor, how the treatment is described and sold, and how society and environment shape our expectations of what a treatment can and cannot do. Many of these examples and their ramifications are discussed in slightly more detail in Ben Goldacre's book Bad Science. * The effect has been observed in animals and babies. However, in practice this requires conditioning first, which is analogous to just telling someone a treatment will work when you can't communicate more directly.^[4]^[5] This is contrary to claims by homeopaths who claim that their remedies work in babies and animals and so cannot be due to the placebo effect. (Homeopathic teething tablets can make babies stop crying, but because they're made of lactose and sucrose (milk and sugar), not because of the homeopathic water.) * Placebos that are "more expensive" work better,^[6] as do ones that come in fancier packaging.^[7] * The effect shows a dose-response relationship like a real medical intervention; two pills are more effective than one. * More dramatic placebos are more effective. Injections work better than pills. A full consultation session with a doctor prior to a placebo is more effective than just giving a patient a pill dismissively. * The placebo effect may possibly have the ability to overpower real pharmacological effects. People who were given very strong drugs to induce nausea didn't develop symptoms as they were told the treatment would actually relieve nausea.^[8] * There are reports from war zones where saline solutions were used due to shortages of morphine, and proved just as effective in battlefield surgery.^[citation needed] * In one trial, the placebo effect was induced even though the individuals were told that it was a placebo that contains no medicine; although they were told that it was shown to be of help to people with such a condition.^[3] * As an indication of the wider cultural meaning and subconscious activity of the effect, it is observed that the colour of a sugar pill also affects the placebo response. Certain colours boost the efficacy of placebos for different illnesses, and can even correlate with gender.^[9] * The placebo effect may actually be getting stronger over time, as expectations about the power of medicine change,^[10] but at the same time it can cause drugs that are old to become weaker as the expectation is that new and novel treatments must be better. * The placebo effect may be transferable; a patient being around someone who believes that the treatment will work can cause the patient to do better - even if that person is a doctor.^[11] This is the reason for double-blinding in drug trials. It may also mean that prayer can affect patients if they know they're being prayed for; rigorous studies of prayer therefore make sure that there is no contact between prayers and patients. * While not related to medicine in and of itself, Tummo meditation is one example of the power of the placebo effect. People practicing it (the most famous being Wim Hof) can keep their body temperature warm even in extremely cold temperatures just by using their mind. [edit] Nocebo effect The placebo effect has a somewhat less well-known, slightly evil cousin; the nocebo effect.^[12]^[13] This occurs when an individual's expectation of negative effects generate those effects, much like a hypochondriac developing symptoms because he or she is actively watching for something to go wrong with their health. This is particularly important when looking at side-effects of drugs, as a placebo pill can generate the negative side-effects associated with drugs, and in some cases, specific side-effects can be increased by mentioning them prominently or adding an extra layer of suggestion, such as complex consent forms.^[14] For ethical reasons the nocebo effect has not been well studied directly, but it is probably behind the witch doctor's ability to cause illness in those people he curses. It has also been used to explain unusual phenomenon like SUNDS (Sudden Unexpected Nocturnal Death Syndrome) in immigrants from the Hmong people of Laos; belief in angry supernatural forces combined with the phenomenon of sleep paralysis to convince sufferers that they were under attack, leading to extremely high levels of stress which could trigger dormant heart defects.^[15] [edit] False placebo A false placebo is a non-placebo factor that appears to cause an improvement of a disease. False placebos differ from actual placebos in that they occur regardless of whether the patient is treated or not. False placebos are the lesser-known things controlled for in the placebo arms of clinical trials. Some examples of false placebos include misdiagnosis, regression to the mean, and spontaneous regression. [edit] See also * Folk remedy * Correlation does not equal causation * Faith healing * Security theatre * Regression to the mean * Spontaneous improvement * Misdiagnosis [edit] Footnotes 1. ↑ Scientific American - Placebo Effect: A Cure in the Mind 2. ↑ Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches, A Branthwaite and P Cooper 3. ↑ ^3.0 ^3.1 Kaptchuk TJ, Friedlander E, Kelley JM, Sanchez MN, Kokkotou E, et al. (2010) Placebos without Deception: A Randomized Controlled Trial in Irritable Bowel Syndrome. PLoS ONE 5(12) 4. ↑ Science Based Medicine - Is There a Placebo Effect for Animals? 5. ↑ Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression, R Ader and N Cohen, Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol 37, Issue 4 333-340 6. ↑ Expensive Placebo Works Better Than Cheap One 7. ↑ Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches, A Branthwaite and P Cooper 8. ↑ Wolf S. Effects of suggestions and conditioning on the action of chemical agents in human subjects - the pharmacology of placebos. J Clin Invest 1950;29:100-9. PMCID PMC439730 9. ↑ Lucchelli, Cattaneo, Zattoni, Effect of capsule colour and order of administration of hypnotic treatments, European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 2004 10. ↑ Placebos Are Getting More Effective. Drugmakers Are Desperate to Know Why, Wired Magazine. 11. ↑ a) Gracely R.H,; Dubner, R.; Deeter, W.R.; Wolksee, P.J., Clinclicians' expectations influence placebo analgesia. Lancet, 1, 43 8419 (1985) b) via Ben Goldacre, Bad Science, p.75. 12. ↑ nocebo effect 13. ↑ New Scientist - when mind attacks body 14. ↑ Bad Science - All bow before the mighty power of the nocebo effect 15. ↑ Adler, Shelley R. Sleep Paralysis. Rutgers: 2011. Retrieved from "http://rationalwiki.org/w/index.php?title=Placebo_effect&oldid=1269184 " Categories: * Psychology * Bronze-level articles * Biology * Medicine * Alternative medicine Hidden category: * Articles with unsourced statements Personal tools * Log in / create account Namespaces * Page * Talk Variants Views * Read * Edit * Fossil record Actions Search ____________________ (BUTTON) Search Navigation * Main page * Recent changes * Random page * Help * Best of RationalWiki * All logs Community * Saloon bar * Forum * To do list * New pages * Technical support * Intercom * Blog Toolbox * What links here * Related changes * Special pages * Printable version * Permanent link support * Donate * This page was last modified on 18 December 2013, at 05:45. * Unless explicitly noted otherwise, all content licensed as indicated by RationalWiki:Copyrights. For concerns on copyright infringement please see: RationalWiki:Copyright violations * Privacy policy * About RationalWiki * Disclaimers * CC-BY-SA 3.0, or any later version * Powered by MediaWiki § [mwol2010_mw_logo_header.gif] 1. Quizzes & Games 2. Word of the Day 3. Video 4. New Words 5. My Favorites View your list of saved words. (You can log in using Facebook.) 1. Dictionary 2. Thesaurus 3. Medical 4. Concise Encyclopedia 5. New! Spanish Central [spanish_central_blue_arrow.gif] Submit placebo effect______ placebo effect placebo effect noun Definition of PLACEBO EFFECT : improvement in the condition of a patient that occurs in response to treatment but cannot be considered due to the specific treatment used First Known Use of PLACEBO EFFECT 1950 Rhymes with PLACEBO EFFECT domino effect, landscape architect placebo effect noun (Medical Dictionary) Medical Definition of PLACEBO EFFECT >> : improvement in the condition of a patient that occurs in response to treatment but cannot be considered due to the specific treatment used Browse Next Word in the Dictionary: place brick Previous Word in the Dictionary: placebo All Words Near: placebo effect [seen-heard-left-quote.gif] Seen & Heard [seen-heard-right-quote.gif] What made you want to look up placebo effect? Please tell us where you read or heard it (including the quote, if possible). View Seen & Heard highlights from around the site » Merriam-Webster on Facebook [name-that-thing-icon-50x50.gif] [quiz-button-94x33.gif] Name That Thing Take Our 10-Question Quiz [hdr_more_quizzes.gif] [spell-it-70x55.gif] Spell It The commonly misspelled words quiz Hear It, Spell It » [vocabulary-quiz-70x55.gif] Vocabulary Quiz How strong is your vocabulary? Take the Quiz » [quizzitive_logo_72x72.jpg] Quizzitive: Our New App! The Vocab Quiz Game for iPhone & iPad [five_stars.jpg] "Incredibly fun and addictive. And informative!" — User Review, iTunes Learn More » [hdr_special_feature_homepage.gif] [collage2013_yiw_resultspg.jpg] The Year in Words Words that spiked in lookups because of events in the news. View the List » [hdr_stay_connected_blue_home.gif] [iphone4_tilt_half.gif] Get Our Free Apps Voice Search, Favorites, Word of the Day, and More iPhone | iPad | Android | More [facebook-logo-300x300.png] [twitter-logo-300x300.png] Join Us on FB & Twitter Get the Word of the Day and More Facebook | Twitter The Merriam-Webster Unabridged Dictionary [unabridged-promo.gif] Online access to a legendary resource Log In or Sign Up » Learning English? We can help. [learners-promo.gif] Visit our free site designed especially for learners and teachers of English LearnersDictionary.com » [iphone-app-woman-with-cell.jpg] Our Dictionary, On Your Devices Merriam-Webster, With Voice Search Get the Free Apps! » Merriam-Webster's Visual Dictionaries [visual-dictionary-promo.gif] The new edition of the remarkable reference features 8,000 illustrations. Learn More » Join Us [twitter-logo-300x300.png] Merriam-Webster on Twitter » [facebook-logo-300x300.png] Merriam-Webster on Facebook » Bookstore: Digital and Print Merriam-Webster references for Mobile, Kindle, print, and more. See all » Other Merriam-Webster Dictionaries Webster's Unabridged Dictionary » WordCentral for Kids » Learner's ESL Dictionary » Visual Dictionary » Spanish Central » Browse the Dictionary 1. Home 2. Help 3. About Us 4. Shop 5. Advertising Info 6. Dictionary API Browse the Thesaurus 1. Privacy Policy 2. About Our Ads 3. Contact Us 4. Browser Tools Browse the Spanish-English Dictionary © Merriam-Webster, Incorporated Browse the Medical Dictionary Browse the Concise Encyclopedia Quantcast § #prev next Skip to main page content * AAN.COM * NEUROLOGY JOURNALS + NEUROLOGY JOURNAL + CLINICAL PRACTICE JOURNAL * CONTINUUM * NEUROLOGY TODAY * NEUROLOGY NOW * AMERICAN BRAIN FOUNDATION * Log in * Activate Subscription * Subscribe * Help * For Residents & Fellows * For Patients * For Authors * For Reviewers * Advanced Search * Search Meeting Abstracts keyword, author ____________________ Submit * Home * Current Issue * All Issues * Ahead of Print * Topics * WriteClick * CME * About the Journal Journal Information * Neurology Journals * Contact Information * Editors & Editorial Board Neurology Resources * AAN Guidelines * Calendar * Translations Services * Subscription Services * Advertise * Permissions Advertisement * * « Previous Article * Table of Contents * Next Article » * Patient Page The placebo effect 1. Joseph H. Friedman, MD and 2. Richard Dubinsky, MD Next Section WHAT IS A CLINICAL RESEARCH TRIAL? In the medical world, a clinical research trial or clinical research study is usually done to prove whether a new medication or therapy is effective, before it becomes an approved and widely used treatment. Patients with the target disease are often asked to take part in clinical research trials. To show that the new treatment is truly effective, it is often compared to a placebo. Previous SectionNext Section WHAT IS A PLACEBO? Placebos are usually thought of as “sugar pills.” They are important “sham” or phony treatments that are used to keep doctors and patients honest in testing new drugs or procedures. To prove that a drug works, it is generally tested against a placebo: a “dummy” medication that should have no effect on the condition. Placebos are not only drugs. Sometimes patients get sham or phony surgery, sham radiation, or some other “pretend” treatment. Many patients and many doctors are unaware of the strength of “mind over matter.” I often tell my patients about a person who was in a study where the drug did not work. He insisted that the drug did work, and that he should be given the drug after the study ended. It turned out that he was not receiving the drug and that he wanted more of the placebo! Previous SectionNext Section WHY ARE PLACEBOS IMPORTANT IN RESEARCH? Doctors and patients both want to see benefit from a new treatment. To prove that a therapy “really” works, some of the patients are given the sugar pill, and some get the drug. Who receives active drug and who receives placebo is often decided by a “coin toss” or a process called randomization. This process makes sure that patients are fairly divided between the two treatments. The change with the real drug is compared to the change with the placebo. The patient and doctors are not told until after the study is over who got the active drug and who got the placebo. Some people wonder whether it is fair or ethical to give a placebo treatment to a patient with a disease and not tell the patient. Many research studies are designed so that all patients eventually are able to use the study medication, even if they do not receive it at first. Studies are carefully reviewed to be sure that use of a placebo treatment does not create a dangerous situation for a patient. In the end, all patients benefit from studies that are carefully done using a placebo, so that the true effect of the treatment can be fully understood. Previous SectionNext Section HOW DO PLACEBOS WORK? How placebos work is still a mystery. It is important to understand that not all placebo effects are good. Just as some patients improve with the power of positive thinking, some get worse and drop out of research studies because of the side effects caused by the placebo. In a recent, well-publicized and fascinating study of Parkinson disease (PD), it was discovered that the patients who improved with placebo had changes in their brain that were identical to the changes caused by the actual medication (called levodopa).^1 Levodopa causes an increase in brain dopamine, and the placebo should not. However, the patients who got better with placebo had a similar increase in dopamine, identical to what happened in those who were given the drug. Talk about mind over matter! That is like convincing yourself you can run a 40-yard dash in 4 seconds and then doing it. Similar effects of changes in brain chemistry have been found in studies of pain and of depression. However, this does not work in secret. When patients in a study of treatment for pain were given pain medications without their knowledge, the benefit was far less than when they were given placebo and they were able to expect a benefit. Previous SectionNext Section WHAT DID THE AUTHORS OF THIS STUDY DISCOVER ABOUT PLACEBOS? Diederich and Goetz^2 brought together some placebo studies in the areas of PD, depression, and pain, and they tried to develop a theory as to why the placebo effect is so large, and how it occurs. They reviewed studies designed to investigate how a placebo produces benefit. The patients with PD who thought that they were receiving the real treatment but who really received a placebo had the same changes in their brains on PET scans as those who received the medication. It was the expectation of the benefit that led to the same chemical response in the placebo group. Similar chemical changes on brain imaging tests were seen with placebo in studies of pain and in studies of depression. In the studies of PD and of pain, the more severe the disease symptoms and the more dramatic the treatment (surgery, injection that the patient can see rather than by a pump), the more likely the subjects were to experience benefit with placebo treatment. Why should brain chemistry change when patients are convinced they are receiving a treatment and their physicians expect them to improve? It seems that the expectation of benefit activates the same natural pathways in the brain as medications. If we could harness these same mechanisms in the clinic, patients could help themselves without the side effects of medications. Previous SectionNext Section WHY ARE THESE STUDIES IMPORTANT? WHAT DOES THE FUTURE HOLD? The authors point out that placebo effects make the study of new treatments very difficult. This is because the effect of a new treatment has to be much greater than the placebo effect, which can be quite large. If the placebo effect is strong, it is harder to prove that a drug or treatment is effective. This means that most research trials have to use many more people than they would if the placebo effect was very small or did not exist at all. This makes the studies cost more and take longer to accomplish. Better understanding of the placebo effect will help us to design faster, more effective studies to better combat disease. Perhaps one day we will also learn how to better harness the positive parts of the placebo effect to use as part of medical therapy. Previous SectionNext Section Footnotes * Series editor: David C. Spencer MD, Section Editor Steven Karceski MD, Section Editor Previous Section REFERENCES 1. 1.↵ de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, et al. Expectation and dopamine release: mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease. Science 2001;293:1164–1166. Abstract/FREE Full Text 2. 2.↵ Diederich NJ, Goetz CG. The placebo treatments in neurosciences: new insights from clinical and neuroimaging studies. Neurology 2008;71:677–684. Abstract/FREE Full Text * Add to Facebook Facebook * Add to LinkedIn LinkedIn * Add to Twitter Twitter What's this? Navigate This Article 1. Top 2. WHAT IS A CLINICAL RESEARCH TRIAL? 3. WHAT IS A PLACEBO? 4. WHY ARE PLACEBOS IMPORTANT IN RESEARCH? 5. HOW DO PLACEBOS WORK? 6. WHAT DID THE AUTHORS OF THIS STUDY DISCOVER ABOUT PLACEBOS? 7. WHY ARE THESE STUDIES IMPORTANT? WHAT DOES THE FUTURE HOLD? 8. Footnotes 9. REFERENCES 1. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000326599.25633.bb Neurology August 26, 2008 vol. 71 no. 9 e25-e26 1. Excerpt 2. » Full Text 3. Full Text (PDF) WriteClick 1. WriteClick now! Submit your comment 2. No WriteClick posts for this article 3. All WriteClick posts 1. Email this article to a colleague 2. Alert me when this article is cited 3. Alert me if a correction is posted 4. Alert me when eletters are published 5. View related articles 6. Find similar articles in PubMed > 7. Add to My Articles & Searches 8. Download to citation manager 9. Request Permissions 1. Load citing article information 2. Citing articles via Google Scholar Google Scholar 1. Articles by Friedman, J. H. 2. Articles by Dubinsky, R. 3. Find similar articles in Google Scholar > PubMed 1. PubMed citation 2. Articles by Friedman, J. H. 3. Articles by Dubinsky, R. 4. Social Bookmarking 1. + Add to Facebook Facebook + Add to LinkedIn LinkedIn + Add to Twitter Twitter What's this? WriteClick Responses to This Article Be the first to respond to this article Respond Related Articles * View related articles» * View more articles about All Clinical trials» * Find similar articles in PubMed > * Find similar articles in Google Scholar > Advertisement * Twitter * Facebook * RSS Feeds + e-Publish Ahead of Print + Current Issue + Recent Issues + Most-Read Articles + Most-Cited Articles + Neurology® Podcast * Mobile Neurology Neurology Print ISSN: 0028-3878 Online ISSN: 1526-632X * American Academy of Neurology * HighWire * [LINK]-Wolters Kluwer Lippincott Williams Wilkins Neurology and WriteClick are trademarks of the American Academy of Neurology. © 2014 American Academy of Neurology | Help | Feedback | Advertise § #RSS Scientific American * Sections + Latest News + Videos + Podcasts + Citizen Science + Bring Science Home + Slide Shows + Energy & Sustainability + Evolution + Health + Mind & Brain + Space + Technology + More Science * Subscribe * SHARE: * * * RT @SciAm Twitter * Share on Google+ Scientific American Magazine Scientific American Mind February 2009 * Health * :: * Features * :: * February 25, 2009 * :: * 13 Comments * :: * Email * :: * Print Cover Image: February 2009 Scientific American Magazine See Inside Placebo Effect: A Cure in the Mind [Preview] Belief is powerful medicine, even if the treatment itself is a sham. New research shows placebos can also benefit patients who do not have faith in them By Maj-Britt Niemi [pin_it_button.png] Image: © ISTOCKPHOTO In Brief * In recent decades reports have confirmed the efficacy of various sham treatments in nearly all areas of medicine. Placebos have helped alleviate pain, depression, anxiety, Parkinson’s disease, inflammatory disorders and even cancer. * Placebo effects can arise not only from a conscious belief in a drug but also from subconscious associations between recovery and the experience of being treated—from the pinch of a shot to a doctor’s white coat. Such subliminal conditioning can control bodily processes of which we are unaware, such as immune responses and the release of hormones. * Researchers have decoded some of the biology of placebo responses, demonstrating that they stem from active processes in the brain. A man whom his doctors referred to as “Mr. Wright” was dying from cancer of the lymph nodes. Orange-size tumors had invaded his neck, groin, chest and abdomen, and his doctors had exhausted all available treatments. Nevertheless, Mr. Wright was confident that a new anticancer drug called Krebiozen would cure him, according to a 1957 report by psychologist Bruno Klopfer of the University of California, Los Angeles, entitled “Psychological Variables in Human Cancer.” Mr. Wright was bedridden and fighting for each breath when he received his first injection. But three days later he was cheerfully ambling around the unit, joking with the nurses. Mr. Wright’s tumors had shrunk by half, and after 10 more days of treatment he was discharged from the hospital. And yet the other patients in the hospital who had received Krebiozen showed no improvement. This article was originally published with the title Cure in the Mind. To read the rest of this article, we recommend purchasing and downloading the digital issue from a computer. Email this article link for reference. [rightsPermsIcon.png] Rights & Permissions Read more: * 2013 Saw Environmental Protests, Regulatory Conflicts of Interest and Lingering Mysteries * SHARE: * * * RT @SciAm Twitter * Share on Google+ Email this Article Placebo Effect: A Cure in the Mind: Scientific American Mind Your Email Address ____________________ Recipient's Email Address (separate multiple addresses with a comma) ____________________ Comment (optional) ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ Send X Search ScientificAme Search View Full site | Subscribe | Subscriber Customer Service | Newsletter Sign Up | Contact Us | Terms of Use | Privacy Policy Scientific American is a trademark of Scientific American, Inc., used with permission © 2014 Scientific American, a Division of Nature America, Inc. All Rights Reserved. § #Science-Based Medicine Science-Based Medicine » Feed Science-Based Medicine » Comments Feed Science-Based Medicine » The Placebo Effect Comments Feed * Home * Contact * Privacy Policy * Website Terms of Use [clear30.png] Science-Based Medicine * About SBM + Editors o Steven P. Novella, MD – Founder and Executive Editor o David H. Gorski, MD, PhD – Managing Editor o Kimball C. Atwood IV, MD o Mark A. Crislip, MD o Harriet Hall, MD o Paul Ingraham – Assistant Editor + Contributors o Steven P. Novella, MD o David H. Gorski, MD, PhD o Kimball C. Atwood IV, MD o Jann Bellamy, JD o Scott Gavura, BScPhm, MBA, RPh o Harriet Hall, MD o Mark A. Crislip, MD + Help with logging in & commenting + Submission Guidelines + SBM Translations * Reference + Acupuncture + Chiropractic + Homeopathy + Vaccines & Autism + Coming Soon o Academics o Cancer Cures o Chelation o Chinese Medicine o Critical Thinking o Herbs & Supplements o Law & Politics o Media & CAM o Naturopathy o Placebo * Links * Recent Comments + Comments (Ungrouped) Articles ____________________ Home → Articles → The Placebo Effect The Placebo Effect Posted by Steven Novella on January 16, 2008 (25 Comments) Recently the Federal Trade Commission went after the makers of the Q-Ray Ionized Bracelet for their claims that their device was a cure for chronic pain. Last week Seventh Circuit judge Frank Easterbrook handed down his opinion on the company’s appeal, writing that the company was guilty of fraud and ordering them to pay 16 million dollars in fines. One of the key points for the company’s defense was that the Q-Ray Ionized Bracelet is legit because it exhibits the placebo effect. Judge Easterbrook was not impressed with this argument, writing: “Like a sugar pill it alleviates symptoms even though there is no apparent medical reason. Since the placebo effect can be obtained from sugar pills, charging $200 for a device that is represented as a miracle cure but works no better than a dummy pill is a form of fraud.” This decision creates an interesting precedent, since there are a large number of fanciful treatments that do not have any “apparent medical” mechanism and that are claimed by its proponents to work through a placebo effect. In my experience the placebo effect, briefly defined as a measurable response to an inert treatment, is almost completely misunderstood by the public – a fact that is exploited by purveyors of dubious treatments such as the Q-ray. Already in the comments of this blog there has been discussion over the nature of the placebo effect. In order to demystify the placebo effect, I will try to first describe exactly what it is. The operational definition of a placebo effect is any health effect measured after an intervention that is something other than a physiological response to a biologically active treatment. In clinical trials the placebo effect is any measured response in the group of study subjects that received an inert treatment, such as a sugar pill. However, “the placebo effect” is a misnomer and contributes to confusion, because it is not a single effect but the net result of many possible factors. The various factors that contribute to a measured or perceived placebo effect vary depending upon the situation – what symptoms or outcomes are being observed. Subjective outcomes like pain, fatigue, and an overall sense of wellbeing, are subject to a host of psychological factors. For example, subjects in clinical studies want to get better, they want to believe they are on the active experimental treatment and that it works, they want to feel that the time and effort they have invested is worthwhile, and they want to make the researchers happy. In turn, the researchers want their treatment to work and want to see their patients get better. So there is often a large reporting bias. In other words, subjects are likely to convince themselves they feel better, and to report that they feel better, even if they don’t. Also, those conducting a trial will tend to make biased observations in favor of a positive effect. It has also been clearly demonstrated that subjects who are being studied in a clinical trial objectively do better. This is because they are in a clinical trial – they are paying closer attention to their overall health, they are likely taking better care of themselves due to the constant reminder of their health and habits provided by the study visits and attention they are getting, they are being examined on a regular basis by a physician, and their overall compliance with treatment is likely to be higher. So basically, subjects in a trial take better care of themselves and get more medical attention than people not in trials. If for those not in a clinical trial, if they decide to do something about their health by starting a new treatment, they are likely to engage in more healthful behavior in other ways. A common belief is that the placebo effect is largely a “mind-over-matter effect,” but this is a misconception. There is no compelling evidence that the mind can create healing simply through will or belief. However, mood and belief can have a significant effect on the subjective perception of pain. There is no method to directly measure pain as a phenomenon, and studies of pain are dependent upon the subjective report of subjects. There is therefore a large potential for perception and reporting bias in pain trials. But there are biological mechanisms by which mental processes can affect pain. There are many non-specific factors that can biochemically suppress pain. For example, increased physical activity can release endorphins that naturally inhibit pain. For these reasons the placebo effect for pain is typically high, around 30%. But the more concrete and physiological the outcome, the smaller the placebo effect. Survival from serious forms of cancer, for example, has no demonstrable placebo effect. There is a “clinical trial effect,” as described above – being a subject in a trial tends to improve care and compliance, but no placebo effect beyond that. There is no compelling evidence that mood or thought alone can help fight off cancer or any similar disease. Other conditions are more objective than pain, but are special because they have a strong influence from the neuro-endocrine system. This system translates psychological stress into physical stress, by releasing stress hormones and increasing activity in the sympathetic nervous system. So, for example, for heart disease mood matters quite a bit. Someone who has an A-type personality and is always angry and upset is at higher risk of a heart attack than someone who is mellow and unstressed. But here there is a known physiological connection between mood and a specific organ – the heart. This cannot be extrapolated to other diseases; it doesn’t mean you can smile your cancer away. Many people talk about the neuroendocrine system’s effect on the immune system. Again, here there is a physiological connection. Stress hormones do suppress the immune system, and it is probably true that extreme stress leaves us physically susceptible to disease for this reason. But the effects of moderate levels of stress are not established. Also, we cannot extrapolate from the risk of getting a cold to the ability to fight off cancer. You have to look at the evidence for each disease unto itself. So while this is a potential contributor, it is overall probably a small effect except in extreme situations. The lack of any real biological placebo effect has led some to question the necessity of having a placebo control in clinical trials. However, I feel there is still a need for a double-blind placebo-controlled design for most clinical trials because that is the only way to minimize the effects of bias on trial outcomes. The factors I listed above will still create the illusion of an effect and the only way to control for this is with a placebo group. Therefore the placebo effect is fairly complex and is largely an artifact of observation and confounding factors. Any real benefits that contribute to the placebo effect can be gained by more straightforward methods – like healthy habits, compliance with treatment, and good health care. The placebo effect is not evidence for any mysterious mind-over-matter effect, but since the mind is matter (the brain) and is connected to the rest of the body, there are some known physiological effects that do play a role (although often greatly exaggerated). In light of all this, I do not feel that knowingly prescribing a placebo treatment is effective or ethical medicine. Modern scientific medicine should strive for interventions that physiologically are scientifically plausible and have sufficient evidence for safety and effectiveness. But I do think there are lessons to be learned from the placebo effect – there are aspects of therapy that do go beyond the physiological intervention. Medicine is not only an applied science; it is the art of humans treating other humans. As part of effective treatment it is helpful to try to maximize all those human intangibles that contribute to a good outcome. But we can do this in the context of scientifically valid treatment, and without crossing the ethically dubious line of deception. I therefore heartily agree with Judge Easterbrook that invoking the placebo effect is not a defense for making fraudulent health claims, for the Q-ray or any other implausible treatment. Posted in: Clinical Trials, Science and Medicine Leave a Comment (25) ↓ 25 thoughts on “The Placebo Effect” 1. PalMD says: January 16, 2008 at 8:53 am I agree that it is ethically impermissible for doctors to prescribe placebos, as it involves actively deceiving a patient. It is *not* ethically unsound to “hype” a treatment, in that if a patient has confidence in a treatment they are more likely to be compliant, etc. For example, I often will tell a patient that the medical regimen for their heart disease will help prevent them from dying, etc (which is true), making them (I hope) more likely to stick to it. I work with residents every day, and many of them have not learned that yet (I’m working on it!). But creating confidence in a treatment/physician/relationship is not placebo—just good medicine. 2. Apreche says: January 16, 2008 at 10:19 am I also agree that it is unethical to prescribe placebos to people who have true illness. However, I am torn on whether it is ethical to prescribe them to hypochondriacs. If someone believes they are ill, when they are not, then perhaps a placebo is a good treatment as long as it is in combination with proper psychological treatment as well. One alternative seems to be to either give them a real treatment for a condition they only believe they have, which is clearly out of the question. The other option seems to be to just send them home telling them they are not ill. That option would seem to result in them turning to pseudoscience for help. I guess that just the psychological treatment alone would be ethically ideal, but since it won’t be effective immediately, it may still result in a turn towards fraudulent treatments. I’m a tech person, not a doctor, so obviously I have no clue what I’m talking about. I’d love to hear what experts have to say about this issue, and that is why I like this blog. 3. daedalus2u says: January 16, 2008 at 12:16 pm As someone who has thought a great deal about the placebo effect in the context of my NO research I have a slightly different perspective. I would distinguish between non-physiological effects, such as investigator bias, and what I consider to be the “true” placebo effect mediated through physiology. I appreciate that this distinction may be difficult (if not impossible) to make experimentally unless specifically looked for (which most trials don’t). In the context of treatment the distinction doesn’t much matter. In the context of clinical trials it can. A single blinding (of patients) might work to correct for the physiological placebo effect mediated though patient physiology, investigator blinding is necessary to correct for investigator bias. In clinical trials double blinding corrects for both, which is why it is the gold standard for clinical trials. I completely agree that prescribing placebos has no place in any treatment modality. There are plenty of lifestyle changes that can be prescribed, diet, exercise, weight loss, stress relief that will do the same things and much more reliably than a placebo medication. Learning how to relax via the relaxation response http://www.mbmi.org/about/default.asp invokes all of these same pathways (as I understand them), and again more reliably than any placebo. I know that my views of the mechanisms and magnitude of the physiological effects of the placebo effect are larger than what is considered “main stream”, however the most I see the placebo effect doing is restoring physiology to the “default” base-line state of normal health. I can think of no mechanism(s), by which the placebo effect would be effective at treating cancer, infections, trauma, or any disorder not caused by chronic stress. Even then it works as a preventative to prevent future damage, not to correct damage already done. 4. BlazingDragon says: January 16, 2008 at 2:00 pm This discussion is interesting and touches on an issue I have strong feelings about. Is someone a hypochondriac or do they have a relatively rare disease (or a rare manifestation of a more common diseaese)? Too many physicians make the assumption, if simple lab tests do not show anything, that the patient is a hypochondriac. They prescribe therapies to get the patients to “shut up,” hoping the “placebo effect” will give the patients relief. This discussion about the placebo effect and how it is unethical to prescribe therapies mostly (or wholly) intended to get a patient to stop calling the doctor miss the fact that it happens often in real practice. Discussions like these are extremely frustrating to me because they talk about an idealized medicine standard that is rarely met these days in actual practice. The time/money constraints put on doctors render ideals pretty much moot for the general public that contacts the health care delivery system these days. I’m all for these discussions on how to make medicine better, but we also need to find ways to make these ideals a much larger part of everyday clinical practice. I firmly believe that clinical trials with double-blinded, placebo-controlled designs are the only validation for therapies. Just because someone “thinks” a therapy might be effective doesn’t make it so, even if the therapy is a new application for an approved drug (see the whole Zetia mess). But Apreche has a very relevant point about people turing to pseudo-science if they feel ignored by the “system.” I watch a lot of Discovery channel shows, so I saw this “ionizing bracelet” crap years ago and couldn’t believe they were selling it (it’s kind of sad how Discovery has to finance themselves with pseudo-science crap because “real” advertisers don’t advertise on “science” channels). I’m glad to see they got their come-uppance and the judge hit on exactly the reason why they are frauds. I wonder how long those japanese toxin-removing foot-pads I’ve seen lately on Discovery will be allowed to sell their crap before they are busted for pseudo-science too (they make even more radical and testable claims about being able to withdraw toxins and heavy metals by sleeping with their special pads stuck to the bottom of your feet). 5. Calli Arcale says: January 16, 2008 at 4:35 pm I have a relative (now long since retired) who used to prescribe placebos for particularly troublesome patients whom he deemed to be hypochondriacs — only they weren’t just sugar pills, they actually were a drug that caused mild nausea. He would warn patients to expect mild nausea as evidence that it was working. They’d go away happy, then come back to report on how well the medicine had worked. When I was younger, I used to think that was a shrewd idea on his part. Now that I’m mature, I find myself wondering if this was part of the reason why he suddenly had to move his practice five hundred miles west (and into another state) just a few years shy of retirement. What he did was actually *worse* than mere deception, since evidently what he gave the patient actually did have an affect, and an unpleasant one at that. 6. Pingback: Placebo–tool or weapon? « WhiteCoat Underground 7. Roy Niles says: January 16, 2008 at 5:26 pm When you talk about treatment “without crossing the ethically dubious line of deception,” are you inferring that deception is ethically dubious per se, or that there is a line between acceptable and unacceptable forms of deception. I would presume you mean the latter, but even so, the standards for determining where that line is crossed might be a good subject for further discussion. 8. psamathos says: January 16, 2008 at 8:54 pm Thanks for the interesting write-up. I also found Mark Crislip’s podcast about the placebo effect informative, if anyone else is looking for more information on misconceptions about the placebo effect: http://www.quackcast.com/QuackCast/Podcasts/Entries/2006/5/22_Quack Cast_5._Placebo_Effect.html 9. daedalus2u says: January 16, 2008 at 9:19 pm I think that any deception in any (purported) therapeutic setting is unethical. I actually think that deception in any setting where there is an expectation of trust is unethical and is a violation of any type of professional conduct. 10. Roy Niles says: January 17, 2008 at 12:29 am There are aspects of deception, both passive and active, that are considered ethical and moral, and that exist in different ways in every profession (and every culture for that matter). It may seem paradoxical, but trust depends on knowing those in that profession understand the difference, and apply that understanding in the best interests of their clients. Problems arise when the “cultural” standards of the particular profession are violated, ignored, twisted, subverted, and when there is no clear agreement as to what those standards should be, and no clear method of enforcing them. 11. Steven Novella says: January 17, 2008 at 10:30 am The question of deception, that I only touched upon in this entry, is a very interesting and important one so I think I will write a thorough treatment of it for my blog entry next week. Stay tuned. 12. daedalus2u says: January 17, 2008 at 6:03 pm For me, my complete unwillingness to be deceptive relates to my knowledge that I do make mistakes. When I am in a position of trust, my mistakes can hurt people who are trusting me and who I don’t want to hurt. Being as truthful with those who are relying on me as I am with myself relieves me of (most) any guilt due to error. 13. daedalus2u says: January 18, 2008 at 8:42 am Another reason I don’t like to be deceptive is because the “best” way to lie is to also lie to yourself that is to believe the lie that you are telling. Figuring out physical reality is difficult enough as it is without injecting lies into it. One you start spreading lies, you start believing lies. When you believe in lies, even “harmless” ones, your conceptualization of reality breaks down and it becomes unreliable. In the context of administering placebos, if you started to do it and saw that they “worked”, it wouldn’t take much rationalization to ascribe mechanisms beyond that of the placebo effect. It is a slippery slope that could take you down into the abyss of quackery. 14. Roy Niles says: January 19, 2008 at 4:24 pm As someone once said, one aspect of deception involves the process of persuading yourself you haven’t just engaged in self-deception. 15. rjstan@together.net says: January 19, 2008 at 7:22 pm I think one of the best ways to start to understand the placebo effect is to speak with scientists who develop drugs or with the stock brokers who follow the industry. The scientists tell tales of experimenting for years on substances that they believed were going to cure horrible diseases only to find out as they tested further that they had been wrong. When you deal with an organism as complex as a living human being, there are many unknown variables which affect the course of a disease. The placebo effect is an umbrella term used to cover them all. While double-blind clinical trials will tell you if a drug is effective, they will not tell you what has caused significant improvement in any particular test subject receiving a placebo. In my experience it is very difficult for many people, especially many people in engineering and the “hard sciences” to accept and deal with the unknown variables in medicine. It is the reason that many are taken in by “alt med” and claims which may sound logical but which cannot be supported by evidence, such as the claim that a supplement that kills bacteria in vitro will also do that in vivo. 16. overshoot says: January 22, 2008 at 12:09 pm rjstan@together.net: “In my experience it is very difficult for many people, especially many people in engineering and the “hard sciences” to accept and deal with the unknown variables in medicine. It is the reason that many are taken in by “alt med” and claims which may sound logical but which cannot be supported by evidence, such as the claim that a supplement that kills bacteria in vitro will also do that in vivo.” I think that very much depends on the “hard science” in question. Certainly physics and electronics do so much in the realm of quantum mechanics that we have no excuse for not appreciating the chaotic nature of complex systems. As for the vitro/vivo issue, please correct me if I’m wrong but IIRC it’s pretty safe to say that an agent that is bactericidal in vitro _will_ also kill them in vivo; it’s just that you can crank up the concentrations in vitro to levels that would be unattainable or intolerable in vivo. My current favorite is the bunch pushing various forms of silver as a magical antibacterial. They promote low concentration systemic use while citing high concentration topical use to argue effectiveness. 17. Pingback: Science Based Medicine » The Ethics of Deception in Medicine 18. Pingback: Transparent Eye » Blog Archive » The Placebo Effect 19. Pingback: Science-Based Medicine » The Role of Anecdotes in Science-Based Medicine 20. Pingback: Science-Based Medicine » Homeopathy and Evidence-Based Medicine: Back to the Future Part V 21. Pingback: NeuroLogica Blog » The Price of Placebos 22. Pingback: Science-Based Medicine » Be Wary of Stem Cell Pseudoscience 23. Pingback: Science-Based Medicine » Studying Placebo Effects 24. Pingback: The Placebo Effect « FreeThought Fort Wayne 25. Pingback: Science-Based Medicine » Placebos in the news again Comments are closed. Categories Select Category[Acupuncture_________________] Archives [Select Month__] Merchandise * SBM e-Books on Amazon * SBM e-Books on iTunes (coming) * SBM e-Books on Nook * SBM on Kindle PayPal - The safer, easier way to pay online! [amazon_kindle_logo.gif] Science-Based Medicine is now available on Kindle * RSS * Twitter * Facebook * Email Recent Posts * The Detox Scam: How to spot it, and how to avoid it * Facebook’s reporting algorithm abused by antivaccinationists to silence pro-science advocates * Reverse Ageing Hype * Doctors Are Not “Only Out to Make Money” * Science-based medicine throughout time follow us in feedly Article Calendar CAPTION: January 2014 M T W T F S S « Dec 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 Read more at SBM Recent Comments or follow our comments in feedly Science-Based Medicine Copyright © 2013 All Rights Reserved. Scroll to Top § #publisher rss Turn autoplay off Turn autoplay on Please activate cookies in order to turn autoplay off * Jump to content [s] * Jump to site navigation [0] * Jump to search [4] * Terms and conditions [8] Edition: UK US AU * Your activity * Email subscriptions * Account details * Linked services Profile Mobile About us * About us, * Contact us * Press office * Guardian Print Centre * Guardian readers' editor * Observer readers' editor * Terms of service * Privacy policy * Advertising guide * Digital archive * Digital edition * Guardian Weekly * Buy Guardian and Observer photos Today's paper * Main section * G2 features * Comment and debate * Editorials, letters and corrections * Obituaries * Other lives * Sport * Film & music * Subscribe Subscribe The Guardian home ____________________ Search * News * Sport * Comment * Culture * Business * Money * Life & style * Travel * Environment * Tech * TV * Video * Dating * Offers * Jobs * News * Science * Placebo effect RSS Placebo effect Most recent * Noma Bar 22032013 George Osborne's sugar pills won't heal the pain, but Julia Gillard's may 21 Mar 2013: Gaby Hinsliff: The placebo effect has its place beyond medicine – as long as those who soothe us do so with our best interests at heart * The placebo effect is present in every medical intervention 21 Mar 2013: Philippa Perry: The western concept of an autonomous self does not account for how a doctor's beliefs can influence the patient's health * Science Weekly podcast: Jim Al-Khalili on the wonders of quantum biology A male robin Audio (50min 20sec), 17 Sep 2012: On this week's show we also hear about new research into the placebo and nocebo effects, and from IgNobel master of ceremonies Marc Abrahams on why great science can be both funny and profound * Subconscious cues can trigger the placebo effect, say researchers 11 Sep 2012: The finding that there are subliminal influences on the placebo effect has implications for how healthcare is delivered * Why I changed my mind about homeopathy 3 Apr 2012: Edzard Ernst: There can be no doubt – people get better after consulting a homeopath, and the reason is clear * Pavlov and placebos could reduce the side-effects of drug treatments Drug capsules 23 Mar 2012: Corrinne Burns: A new study suggests patients can be conditioned to respond to a placebo in place of a drug, allowing doses to be reduced * The nocebo effect: Wellcome Trust science writing prize essay 13 Nov 2011: Penny Sarchet discusses research on the 'nocebo' effect in her winning essay for the Wellcome Trust science writing prize, awarded in association with the Guardian and Observer * Detox: flushing out poison or absorbing dangerous claptrap? 29 Aug 2011: The alternative-medicine version of detox has real-world side-effects that outweigh its imaginary benefits * Is Avram Grant football's placebo effect? avram grant 14 Jul 2011: Harry Pearson: Scientists may have solved the mystery of how Avram Grant keeps getting top-level coaching jobs * Placebos may help us but they don't have the same effect as medicine 16 Mar 2011: Response: There's a lot of confusion over the nature of the inert dummy pills used in drug trials, says Tom Burns * Half of all German doctors prescribe placebos, new study shows 6 Mar 2011: Placebo cures shown to help with depression and stomach complaints – in Bavaria, 88% of doctors have prescribed them * How security can be more theatre than reality UK Border Agency drug detection 26 Feb 2011: Ben Goldacre: Drug-detecting dogs and hi-tech brain scans to spot liars – examples of how we are still buying into the placebo effect * Placebos, again 21 Feb 2011: Andrew Brown: Placebos work because people believe in them. So what do we make of a study that shows they work even when patients are told they are sugar pills? * The strange powers of the Placebo Effect 15 Feb 2011: Hypothesis: some people experience relief of some health symptoms simply by believing that a substance will make them feel better * Celebrity endorsements that are science fiction trashed in annual list A Power Balance silicone bracelet containing a hologram 29 Dec 2010: Campaigners name and shame stars for dodgy health and diet claims * Next * 1 * 2 * 3 1-15 of 36 for Placebo effect Related subjects Science * Medical research * Science and scepticism * Neuroscience * Controversies * Drugs * Show five more…… * Psychology * Biology * Physics * Science writing prize * Zoology Society * Health * Depression * Doctors * Mental health * MMR * Show three more…… * Midwifery * NHS * Organ donation Life and style * Health & wellbeing * Alternative medicine * Acupuncture * Homeopathy * Celebrity * Show two more…… * Childbirth * Pregnancy World news * Religion * Germany * Animals * Asia Pacific * Australia * Show three more…… * Australian politics * Europe * Julia Gillard Education * Higher education * Science * Medicine * Research More Blogs * The placebo paradox Andrew Brown, 6 Sep 2009 15.19 BST * Homeopathy: Sometimes a dose of nothing can do you a power of good Michael Brooks, 6 Feb 2009 14.49 GMT * Science Weekly: The Placebo Effect Ben Green, 3 Mar 2008 13.54 GMT Today's best video * Roger Federer smash shot leaves opponents baffled at Brisbane Invitational - video Did Roger Federer intend to do this incredible 'trick' smash shot? Former world No1 leaves opponents and partner bemused during doubles match in Australia * Helicopter evacuation to the Aurora Australis Antarctic rescue mission a success scientists and passengers airlifted to the Aurora Australis by a Chinese helicopte * Giant yellow duck explodes hours before New Year's Eve in Taiwan - video Giant yellow duck explodes in Taiwan Dutch artist's creation explodes in a Taiwan port * Bushfires rage in North Stradbroke Island Australian temperatures hit 54C Fire crews battle bushfires on North Stradbroke Island, Queensland * The NSA files trailblock image Live: Follow NSA-related developments as controversy over leaks continues to make headlines On Science * Most viewed * Latest Last 24 hours 1. [BATMAN-004.jpg] 1. Scientists tell us their favourite jokes: 'An electron and a positron walked into a bar…' 2. 2. Sherlock Holmes is the archetypal scientist – brilliant but slightly scary 3. 3. The Aurora Australis, our new home after Antarctic rescue – in pictures 4. 4. The friendly mind-reading robot pet – video 5. 5. Happy New Year: an illogical, unreasonable statement 6. More most viewed Last 24 hours 1. [Helicopter-on-ice-003.jpg] 1. Antarctic rescue: free at last! 2. 2. Antarctic passengers rescued from ship by helicopter - live coverage 3. 3. The Aurora Australis, our new home after Antarctic rescue – in pictures 4. 4. Antarctica: crew evacuated from stranded ship via helicopter - video 5. 5. The friendly mind-reading robot pet – video 6. All today's stories Guardian Bookshop This week's bestsellers 1. Cold 1. Cold by Ranulph Fiennes £16.00 2. 2. Nothing £6.39 3. 3. Simpsons and Their Mathematical Secrets by Simon Singh £15.19 4. 4. Signal and the Noise by Nate Silver £7.19 5. 5. Numbers Game by Chris Anderson £10.39 Search the Guardian bookshop ____________________ (BUTTON) Search Sponsored feature * License/buy our content | * Privacy policy | * Terms & conditions | * Advertising guide | * Accessibility | * A-Z index | * Inside the Guardian blog | * About us | * Work for us | * Join our dating site today * © 2014 Guardian News and Media Limited or its affiliated companies. All rights reserved. Quantcast § #ICRA labels * Skip to main content * Skip to main navigation * Help with accessibility * Home * About * Contact * Tools * Video * Choose and Book * Communities * IPS * Translate * Log in or * create an account Go to NHS Choices homepage Your health, your choices Information navigation Search entire site Enter a search term: Enter a search term_ Search Main navigation * Health A-Z + Hundreds of conditions explained + Arthritis information + Asthma information + Back pain information + Stress, anxiety, depression information + Breast cancer information + Pregnancy and baby information + Diabetes information + Dementia information + Heart disease information + Measles information + Flu information + Vaccinations information + Online clinics information + Common health questions + Symptom checkers + Medicines A-Z + All A-Z topics * Live Well + Over 100 topics on healthy living + Alcohol articles + Couch to 5K articles + Winter health articles + Fitness articles + The flu jab articles + Healthy eating articles + Lose weight articles + Mental health articles + Pain articles + Sexual health articles + Sleep articles + Stop smoking articles + Tiredness articles + Health check tools + All Live Well topics * Care and support + Your essential guide to social care + About social care + Choosing care services + Social care assessments + NHS continuing care + Mental capacity + Carers’ wellbeing + Home care + Care homes + Breaks from caring + Carers’ rights + Young carers + All care and support topics * Health news + Health news stories unspun + Diet and nutrition news reports + Obesity and weight loss news reports + Neurology and dementia news reports + Lifestyle and environment news reports + Pregnancy and children news reports + Cancer news reports + Drugs and vaccines news reports + Heart and lungs news reports + Medical practice news reports + Older people and ageing news reports + Genetics and stem cells news reports + Mental health news reports + Diabetes news reports + Topical questions and answers + Special reports + All Behind the Headlines news * Services near you + its your choice Don't miss out ... Exercise your right to choice in the NHS Learn about patient choice now + A&E search + Hospitals search + GPs search + Dentistssearch + Pharmacies search + Care homes and care at home search + Alcohol search + Maternity search + Consultants search + Opticians search + Sexual health search + Stop smoking services + Urgent care services + All directories The placebo effect Share: * Email share * Twitter share * Facebook share Save: * Google Bookmarks * NHS Choices Saved Pages Subscribe: RSS feed Print: * View all 116 topics Popular topics * Alcohol * Colds and flu * Contraception * Couch to 5K * Fitness * Healthy Christmas * Healthy eating * Lose weight * Sexual health * Stop smoking * Strength and Flex * Stress, anxiety and depression * Tiredness and fatigue * Weight loss guide * Winter health * View all 116 topics * Complementary and alternative medicine Special reports * Class of 1948 * Military and veterans' health * NHS at 65 * Olympics * On a budget Child health * Babies and toddlers * Your child's health 6-15 * Adoption and fostering Men's health * Teen boys 15-18 * Men's health 18-39 * Men's health 40-60 * Men's health 60-plus Women's health * Teen girls 15-18 * Women's health 18-39 * Women's health 40-60 * Women's health 60-plus The placebo effect When a person uses any type of health treatment and sees an improvement in their symptoms, they may be experiencing the placebo effect. That’s why it’s important to be aware of the placebo effect when judging the effectiveness of a treatment, or when using one ourselves. The placebo effect is about the power of the mind to influence the body. It can occur when a person uses any kind of health treatment, either conventional or complementary and alternative. It can affect all of us, whether we know about the placebo effect or not. It’s important to be aware of the placebo effect when choosing complementary and alternative treatments. That is because if we choose a complementary or alternative treatment that does not work – and causes only a placebo effect – we may miss out on more effective treatments. What is the placebo effect? For hundreds of years, doctors have known that when a patient with a health condition expects their symptoms to improve, they often do improve. Today, we know that patients who are given empty injections or pills that they believe contain medicine can experience improvement in a wide range of health conditions. This kind of fake or empty medicine is often called a placebo, and the improvement that results is called the placebo effect. The placebo effect is an example of how our expectations and beliefs can cause real change in our physical bodies. It’s a phenomenon that we don’t completely understand. But we can see it working in all kinds of ways, and all kinds of circumstances. The placebo effect at work Take one well-known example based on a physical feeling we are all familiar with: pain. In 1996, scientists assembled a group of students and told them that they were going to take part in a study of a new painkiller, called 'trivaricaine'. Trivaricaine was a brown lotion to be painted on the skin, and smelled like a medicine. But the students were not told that, in fact, trivaricaine contained only water, iodine, and thyme oil, none of which are painkilling medicines. It was a fake – or placebo – painkiller. Read an abstract of the study: Mechanisms of Placebo Pain Reduction. With each student, the trivaricaine was painted on one index finger, and the other left untreated. In turn, each index finger was squeezed in a vice. The students reported significantly less pain in the treated finger, even though trivaricaine was a fake. In this example, expectation and belief produced real results. The students expected the 'medicine' to kill pain: and, sure enough, they experienced less pain. This is the placebo effect. Placebo medicine has even been shown to cause stomach ulcers to heal faster than they otherwise would. These amazing results show that the placebo effect is real, and powerful. They mean that fake or placebo treatments can cause real improvements in health conditions: improvements we can see with our own eyes. Experiencing the placebo effect is not the same as being 'tricked', or being foolish. The effect can happen to everyone, however intelligent, and whether they know about the placebo effect or not. CAM and the placebo effect Evidence about a treatment is gathered by conducting fair tests. In these tests, scientists find out whether a treatment causes an improvement beyond the improvement caused by the placebo effect alone. Evidence plays an important role in mainstream medicine. This means that when you use many conventional medicines, you can be sure that there is evidence that they work. When patients experience improvement after using a healthcare treatment that has not been proven to work, they may be experiencing only the placebo effect. Of course, improvement in a health condition due to the placebo effect is still improvement, and that is always welcome. But it is important to remember that for many health conditions, there are treatments that work better than placebo treatments. If you choose a treatment that provides only a placebo effect, you will miss out on the benefit that a better treatment would provide. Check the evidence The only way to know whether a health treatment works better than a placebo treatment is by checking the evidence. You can learn more about evidence, how it is gathered, and why it is important in CAM: what is evidence?. You can learn about the evidence for many of the best-known complementary and alternative medicines in the Health A-Z pages. Page last reviewed: 23/11/2012 Next review due: 23/11/2014 More articles on: Complementary and alternative medicine Share: * Email share * Twitter share * Facebook share Save: * Google Bookmarks * NHS Choices Saved Pages Subscribe: RSS feed Print: Ratings How helpful is this page? Average rating [Helpful_________] Based on 16 ratings All ratings [Very helpful____] 12 ratings [Helpful_________] 1 ratings [Fairly helpful__] 1 ratings [Not very helpful] 0 ratings [Unhelpful_______] 2 ratings Add your rating [Very helpful____] Submit rating Leave your comment Login or Register Share your views and experiences with others. If you want a response from an NHS professional or the website team, please contact us. Comments The 1 comments about ‘The placebo effect’ posted are personal views. Any information they give has not been checked and may not be accurate. rationallawful said on 04 February 2013 You rightly say 'of course, improvement in a health condition due to the placebo effect is still improvement, and that is always welcome'; however sadly certain scientists who should know better wrongly equate 'placebo' with 'invalid' and 'contrary to evidence based medicine' and then go on to bad-mouth complementary therapies including homeopathy which they say should be banned. The greatest strength of the NHS is that it is COMPREHENSIVE - see the 2012 Health and Social Care Act (Section 1.1): 'The Secretary of State must continue the promotion in England of a comprehensive health service designed to secure improvement— .(a) in the physical and mental health of the people of England, and (b) in the prevention, diagnosis and treatment of physical and mental illness’. The time has come to find a better word than 'Placebo' which generates more heat than light. It wraps up a lot of heterogeneous concepts into a single word which is then meaningless in a scientific sense. This point has been taken up in the July 2012 in-depth (294 page) doctoral dissertation 'Evidence-based medicine, "placebos" and the homeopathy controversy' by Dr Andrew James Turner of the University of Nottingham at http://etheses.nottingham.ac.uk/2577/ which shows (page 185) that lumping a disparate range of elements together only adds to the confusion; if one wishes to say something informative about medical treatments, ‘placebo’ and ‘placebo effect’ are not terms which are analytically useful. Better to talk about 'components of treatment' one or more of which can contribute towards the effectiveness of a therapy. A growing body of published research in good quality peer-reviewed journals shows that homeopathy has a positive effect. Healthcare is about adding years to life and life to years and homeopathy when practised (as it is)and integrated by qualified medical practitioners time and time again demonstrates that it is economic al, safe, and effective. Report this content as offensive or unsuitable comment id 33818 Getting started * Learn how medicine is regulated * Look up any approved medicine * Browse common health questions * Learn how to decipher media health claims * Ask your pharmacist for advice Services near you Find addresses, phone numbers and websites for services near you Find and compare services Location Enter a postcode or Find services Looking for other services? View services A-Z Also on NHS Choices * About acupuncture * About chiropractic * About homeopathy * About osetopathy * About hypnotherapy * Are complementary medicines safe in pregnancy? * About clinical trials and medical research CAM: what is evidence? What is scientific evidence and how can you use it when deciding whether to use complementary and alternative medicine? About complementary and alternative medicine All about complementary and alternative medcine, including what it is, how it is regulated, how to choose a practitioner and what evidence there is You be the judge Fact or fiction? Killer or cure? We show you how to look Behind the Headlines The placebo effect Viewing video content in NHS Choices If you do not have a version of the Flash Player you can download the free Adobe Flash Player from Adobe Systems Incorporated. Ben Goldacre explains what the placebo effect is and describes its role in medical research and in the pharmaceutical industry. Media last reviewed: 02/10/2013 Next review due: 02/10/2015 * NHS Choices information * Choices e-newsletters * Your pages Site policies * Terms and conditions * Editorial policy * Comments policy * Syndication * Privacy policy * Cookies policy * Links policy * Personal accounts * Accessibility * Sitemap Other NHS sites * Choose and Book * Summary Care Records * NHS Direct * NHS Scotland * NHS Northern Ireland * NHS Wales * NHS Careers * NHS Jobs * Department of Health * NHS England About the NHS * History of the NHS * About NHS services * Choice in the NHS * Quality accounts * PROMs * Find authorities and trusts * Healthwatch England Other channels * Follow us on Twitter * Facebook * YouTube * Video library * Links library * NHS Choices Training Other Languages Visit our language section for more health websites in foreign languages. Contact NHS Choices * Choices helpdesk * Freedom of Information requests $feature.FeatureMainImageAltText NHS Choices e-newsletters Sign up for Your Health, the monthly e-newsletter packed with the latest news and topical tips from NHS Choices NHS Choices e-newsletters Sign up Emails from NHS Choices NHS Choices offers a range of e-newsletters on various topics. Sign up now for the Your Health newsletter to get monthly news and advice straight to your inbox Create an NHS Choices account With an account you can keep track of pages on the site and save them to this tab, which you can access on every page when you are logged in. Create account Already have an account? Log in * Saved pages Keep track of important pages * Recently visited pages Easily find again pages you have been reading * Pages you might like Have pages recommended to you * – Link to gov.uk – The new place to find government services and information * – The Information Standard - Certified member § #publisher alternate Better Health Channel on twitter Connect with us via Twitter and share Australia's best health and medical info with those close to you Close survey * About Us * Contact Us * Events * Newsletter * Sitemap * A-Z Better Health Channel Better Health Channel fact sheet * Better Health Channel on facebook Better Health Channel on twitter Better Health Channel on google+ Better Health Channel on google+ * Follow us: Search this site ____________________ Search * Home * Conditions & Treatments Illness, prescription drugs, first aid, natural rem... * Healthy Living Food & fitness, prevention, ... * Relationships & Family Relationships, sex, pregnancy, parental guid... * Services & Support Find a doctor, hospital, suppo... Home > Conditions and treatments > Medications - Introduction to medications > Placebo effect Placebo effect * Listen * Print this page Print * AAA Font size Placebo effect Summary A placebo is any medical treatment that is inert (inactive), such as a sugar pill. Around one third of people who take placebos (believing them to be medication) will experience an end to their symptoms. This is called the placebo effect. Download the PDF version of this fact sheet Email this fact sheet Placebo is Latin for 'I will please', and refers to any medical treatment that is inert. The placebo has long been used in research trials to objectively test the efficacy of a new health care treatment, such as a drug. A placebo is indispensable to the conduct of many scientifically-based clinical trials. Ethical considerations require that participants in clinical trials be told that they may be given a 'dummy' treatment. Usually, one group of people takes the drug while another group (the control group) takes the placebo. The placebo may be a sugar pill. None of the participants know whether they are taking the active or inactive substance. Sometimes, not even the researchers know (double blind test). Comparing the results from both groups should indicate the effects of the drug. However, people sometimes get better when they are taking a placebo. This phenomenon is known as 'the placebo effect'. Estimates vary, but around one third of people taking placebos for complaints (including pain, headache and seasickness) will experience relief from symptoms. There are various theories that attempt to explain this phenomenon but the underlying mechanisms remain mysterious. Types of placebos A placebo doesn't have to be a pill. It can be any inert or 'dummy' treatment, such as special diets, exercise, physical therapy or even surgery. The placebo effect is triggered by the person's belief in the treatment and their expectation of feeling better, rather than the specific form the placebo takes. The misconception of 'imaginary' disorders If a person's symptoms are relieved by taking an inert substance or undergoing a dummy procedure, it seems logical to assume that their illness must have been imaginary. This is not the case. Medical research has shown that psychological states play an important role in the development of disease. For example, stress is known to increase blood pressure, and chronic hypertension is a risk factor for heart disease. Just as the mind can contribute to a physical disorder, it can also contribute to its cure. Influencing factors Some of the factors that influence the placebo effect include: * The characteristics of the placebo - if the pill looks genuine, the person taking it is more likely to believe that it contains medicine. Research shows that larger sized pills suggest a stronger dose than smaller pills, and taking two pills appears more potent than swallowing just one. Generally, injections have a more powerful effect than pills. * The person's attitude - if the person expects the treatment to work, the chances of a placebo effect are higher. Some studies show that the placebo effect may still take place even if the person is sceptical of success. The power of suggestion may be at work here. * Doctor-patient relationship - if the person trusts their health care practitioner, they are more likely to believe that the placebo will work. How placebos work The exact physiological mechanisms remain mysterious. Some of the theories that attempt to explain the placebo effect include: * Self-limiting disorders - many conditions, such as the common cold, are self-limiting. They will resolve by themselves anyway, with or without placebos or drugs, and the end of symptoms is merely coincidence. * Remission - the symptoms of some disorders, such as multiple sclerosis and lupus, may wax and wane. A remission during a course of placebos may be coincidence, and not due to the placebos at all. * A change in behaviour - the placebo may increase a person's motivation to take better care of themselves. Improved diet, regular exercise or rest may be responsible for the easing of their symptoms. * Altered perception - the person's interpretation of their symptoms may change with the expectation of feeling better. For example, a sharp pain may be reinterpreted as an uncomfortable tingling. * Reduced anxiety - taking the placebo and expecting to feel better may soothe the autonomic nervous system and reduce the levels of stress chemicals, such as adrenaline. * Brain chemicals - placebos may trigger the release of the body's own natural painkillers, the brain chemicals (neurotransmitters) known as endorphins. * Altered brain state - research indicates that the brain responds to an imagined scene in much the same way as it responds to an actual visualised scene. A placebo may help the brain to remember a time before the onset of symptoms, and then bring about physiological change. This theory is called 'remembered wellness'. The argument against placebos Placebos have the power to cause unwanted side effects. Nausea, drowsiness and allergic reactions, such as skin rashes, have been reported as placebo effects. Critics of placebos maintain that deception is wrong, regardless of whether the deceived patient experiences an end to their symptoms. The longterm prescription of placebos by an unscrupulous health care practitioner may also convince an otherwise healthy person that they are suffering from an illness or infection that requires ongoing treatment. The mind-set of believing oneself to be ill may contribute to the onset of genuine symptoms. A good doctor-patient relationship with good communication is usually preferable to the use of a placebo. The placebo effect is doubted by some Some researchers doubt the placebo effect even exists. For example, medical philosophers at the University of Copenhagen recently analysed trials that involved placebos and declared that placebos offered about the same degree of symptom relief as no medical treatment. Critics point out that the methodology used by the medical philosophers was flawed, and that the placebo effect has been well documented for many years. Where to get help * Your doctor Things to remember * A placebo is any medical treatment that is inert, such as a sugar pill. * Around one third of people who take placebos (believing them to be medication) will experience an end to their symptoms. * Belief in a treatment may be enough to change the course of a person's physical illness. You might also be interested in: + Clinical trials. Want to know more? Go to More information for support groups, related links and references. This page has been produced in consultation with and approved by: Monash University - Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology (Logo links to further information) Monash University - Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology Fact sheet currently being reviewed. Last reviewed: October 2011 Content on this website is provided for education and information purposes only. Information about a therapy, service, product or treatment does not imply endorsement and is not intended to replace advice from your doctor or other registered health professional. Content has been prepared for Victorian residents and wider Australian audiences, and was accurate at the time of publication. Readers should note that, over time, currency and completeness of the information may change. All users are urged to always seek advice from a registered health care professional for diagnosis and answers to their medical questions. If you would like to link to this fact sheet on your website, simply copy the code below and add it to your page: Placebo effect - Better Health Channel
A placebo is any medical treatment that is inert (inactive), such as a sugar pill. Around one third of people who take placebos (believing them to be medication) will experience an end to their symptoms. This is called the placebo effect. Content on this website is provided for education and information purposes only. Information about a therapy, service, product or treatment does not imply endorsement and is not intended to replace advice from your qualified health professional. Content has been prepared for Victorian residence and wider Australian audiences, and was accurate at the time of publication. Readers should note that over time currency and completeness of the information may change. All users are urged to always seek advice from a qualified health care professional for diagnosis and answers to their medical questions. For the latest updates and more information, visit www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au Copyight (c) 1999/2014 State of Victoria. Reproduced from the Better Health Channel (www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au) at no cost with permission of the Victorian Minister for Health. Unauthorised reproduction and other uses comprised in the copyright are prohibited without permission. footer image for printing Find a health service In Victoria, Australia Where ______________________________ e.g. Collingwood VIC or 3066 Service [-- Health Professional --] Find Loading Searching... For immediate, expert health advice from a registered nurse, call: Nurse on call: 1300 60 60 24 More information * Content partner * More information Keep up to date Email newsletter Receive BHC health news See current newsletter Email ____________________ Subscribe RSS What is RSS? Receive RSS updates Follow us on Twitter What is twitter? [INFO] Did you know the odds of #conceiving a baby are higher at certain times in a woman's cycle? More: http://t.co/uamnZmhSzN #fertility 02/09/2013 02:30:05 AM [INFO] Many things can cause a bout of #gastro? More on symptoms, treatment & prevention: http://t.co/h8bxw1Qk49 01/09/2013 03:45:03 AM [INFO] Want to know how to treat #gastro in young children? Here's how: http://t.co/epVophcuXe #kidshealth 01/09/2013 03:40:03 AM Follow BetterHealthGov on Twitter Feedback * del.cio.us * Share Share * twitter * Better Health Channel on google+ * Terms and conditions * Privacy (c) 2014 State Government of Victoria Last updated 10/31/2013 § Google+ #brain Feed (RSS) brain Feed (Atom) idea explorer Feed (RSS) idea explorer Feed (Atom) medicine Feed (RSS) medicine Feed (Atom) mind-bending Feed (RSS) mind-bending Feed (Atom) Radiolab Podcast * Stations * About * Hi, * Logout * Sign In ____________________ Search * Facebook * Twitter Donate Now! Radiolab * Listen * Read * Watch * Search * Support Radiolab * About Navigate * Listen * Read * Watch * Search * Support Radiolab * About Donate Return Home Season 3 | Episode 1 Placebo « previous episode | next episode » Jacob Smullyan * Share * Tweet * * Print * Email * Stream m3u Pill Pill (Lucas Lucas/flickr/CC-BY-2.0) With new research demonstrating the startling power of the placebo effect, this hour of Radiolab examines the chemical consequences of belief and imagination. * Podcasts More Could the best medicine be no medicine at all? We take stock of the pharmacy in our brains, consider the symbolic power of the doctor coat, and visit the tent of a self-proclaimed faith healer. Guests: Dr. Naji Abumrad, Fabrizio Benedetti, preacher Steve Buza, Dr. Daniel Carr, Ed Cohen, Ann Harrington, Dr. Albert Mason, Daniel Moerman and Tor Wager Tags: * brain * idea explorer * medicine * mind bending pills Pinpointing the Placebo Effect All over the world, people say they are healed by things that turn out to be placebo. So it's easy to think that they must have been faking in the first place if all it took was a little sugar pill to assuage their ailments. But keep your scoffing at ... Comments [22] white lab coat The White Coat Next up: a look at the placebo effect from the doctor's perspective. How the medical context alone can be the key into the brain's healing resources. We'll hear the story of Dr. Albert Mason, who found he had super-powers, used them for good, and then lost them forever. Then, we'll ... Comments [9] hypnotizing cartoon Faith Healers The very first placebo-controlled trial may have been the debunking of the charismatic Anton Mesmer (the enigmatic source responsible for the verb “to mesmerize”), an enlightenment figure with a healing technique that Ben Franklin, for one, thought was basically placebo performance. Historians Ed Cohen ... Comments [19] Related * Leave a comment * RSS RSS Feed for Comments Comments [43] Lindsay L from Oceanside, CA Is there a transcript available for this episode? Nov. 15 2013 08:56 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Laura from Indianapolis, IN I would be interested in someone covering the placebo effect as it relates to the sponge and water that seems to be a panacea for all ailments in international soccer games. I have always been amazed at the power of the sponge to immediately regurgitate a soccer player writhing about on the pitch. Sep. 03 2013 01:52 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Amanda from NYC Does the audio play for anyone? I can't stream it, and when I tried to download it, the file had zero bytes. I listened to this one a few years ago and wanted to re-listen. Aug. 21 2013 11:49 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate L W Calhoun from Atlanta The placebo effect is a form of mind control, and as such, its effects can be either positive or negative. Something works because someone believes it works. Placebo is why witchcraft works. It’s real for those who believe it’s real. Jun. 26 2013 05:03 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Daniel from Jacksonville, Fl. I am a christian and I always find "healing" very suspicious. I do believe in the power of faith and I believe in the power of God. I also believe in a God that has the ability to heal and that perhaps people through faith can be healed of all kinds of illness. But to state that sickness is the devil living in you or that one may be sick because of lack of faith is unbiblical. This belief is just another silly thing that a small amount of christians believe but seems to get a lot of media attention, mainly because it is so mindless (a common theme that the media portrays Christians to be). We live in a world full of disease caused from all different things not because some one is possessed or the devil is attached to their spine and causes a curve. I would encourage people to take that segment lightly as the majority of Christians would not agree with what was reported on. Every church I have ever attended would never encourage someone to substitute medicine with prayer. Prayer is for faith, to grow closer to God, not to replace a doctor. Jun. 25 2013 02:19 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate ted from TX On the placebo effect where you compared soldiers to ordinary folks. The soldiers were generally much younger (recruits) and have been in physical training for a year or two, andthey have had it drilled into their heads that nthey may be shot. They were conditioned fore th epain. On the other hand the ordinasry peoplewere probably older, not in great physical shape, and they were not at all perpared for being shot. I believe this Radio Lab story was sophomoric. Jun. 23 2013 09:04 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Dave Vallee from Mahanoy City It is troubling, and saddening to hear a religious ritual occurring in a medical training facility. Well, to hear it anywhere really. I wonder if it would have invoked comment from the producers if the prayers had been offered to Zeus or Thor. Likewise, you should have felt compelled to make some inquiry into the belief system of the woman who experienced the alleged faith healer. Yes, you were trying to maintain continuity of context, but the difference in how we form the stories that make possible the interactions of medical personnel, and those of charlatans should have been distinguished by your reporting. One way to have done that would have been to simply inquire of that woman if she wondered why Christ died for cures, and not for preventions. Oversight? Jun. 23 2013 04:26 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Candace Clemens from Arlington, MA Prof. Riffs - I was at a UC medical conference where an award was presented to the doctor whose researched proved for certain the efficacy of the placebo effect. This was after a doctor received an IgNobel award for the same conclusion. The power of the mind is the new frontier in science, and that is why all neurologists and psychiatrists need to study both. Nobody knows for sure how to distinguish between neurological impulses and behavioral manifestation. http://psychologyofpain.blogspot.com/2008/10/ig-nobel-prize-for-study-o n-placebo.html Jun. 22 2013 11:00 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate alan from atlanis Before the library of Alexandria was burned there was just as much reason as when Ben Franklin started playing with kites. We just forgot. Check out the ancient discoveries series. there are six episodes this one is about robotics https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_TFN3aAPJLU Jun. 22 2013 06:35 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Kristy RE: the doctor who lost his confidence after a respected elder physician told him his claim of curing the boy of his skin problem was impossible: My mother had a heart attack in 1974 and the cardiologist who treated her told her that she had about 10 years to live. (I found out from my sister after she died). She had a heart attack and died two days past the 10th anniversary of her initial heart attack. (The kind of which is routinely treated in emergency rooms today). I have long attributed this to the fact that my mother, not having graduated high school, put a lot of trust in people who were more educated than she. In effect, the doctor said she had 10 years, and she did as she was told. Regarding the warts---I know someone who, as a child, had warts and they disappeared after she gave them to her friend (!) as per her doctor's instruction. Soon her warts were gone and her friend had them! Jun. 21 2013 11:00 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: -1/1 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate John Parsons from Durham, NH During the discussion of the color of sleeping pills, it was mentioned that in every country in the world, blue sleeping pills work for men - except in Italy. The explanation was that the Italian national football team is called The Blues (Gli Azzurri) and that the color represented the excitement of a game, thus preventing sleep. The problem with that reasoning is that right next door, in France, the national football team is also called The Blues (Les Bleus), so why do the pills work there? The French are just as passionate about their football as the Italians. Somewhere there must be a different reason for the failure of the pills in Italian men. Jun. 21 2013 12:43 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate JustSomeone JustSomeone I found the faith healing section very hard to listen to. I have scoliosis and hearing that it was "a sign of the devil" upset me to the point that I now avoid this episode. I don't think its bad! I personally just find it hard to listen to. May. 30 2013 12:16 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: -1/1 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Lou Vignates Consider these terms together: "placebo effect," "spontaneous remission," "will to live," "expectation of getting better," and then consider that you don't explain anything by giving it a name. Aug. 27 2012 06:31 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate joyce WISH THERE WAS A PLACEBO FOR HOT FLASHES!!!! Feb. 26 2012 04:06 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Kate harvard just started a program to research the placebo effect, website is www.programinplacebostudies.org Jan. 06 2012 11:54 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Craftylady Craftylady I love the fact of the history on this topic.Medical trials still go on today with the placebo.Are you scared or is it your belief to judge this episode ? Dec. 18 2011 12:01 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Professor Riffs Maybe the worst episode of Radiolab. The placebo effect is quackery extreme... I can see no reason to give it this much positive attention. Oct. 08 2011 02:44 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: -4/4 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Aimee from Perth, Australia I laughed when I heard the story of the lady's faith healing: I know how that trick works. You can try it yourself. First, the control: form an "O" between you thumb and forefinger, with your wrist straight. Try to pull through your thumb and forfinger with the other hand - I know I can't. Now form the "O" with your wrist bent at 90 degrees; this time it's easy to pull through the thumb and forefinger. I'd bet a large Easter egg that that's how the faith healer did it. When the wrist is bent, the finger grip is significantly weaker. Source: Practicing Aikido regularly. Sep. 29 2011 05:01 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate l_e_cox l_e_cox Finally! We sail beyond the domain of the merely scientific! This is the stuff leaps in knowledge are made of! But don't let anyone convince you that "we don't know why this works." We know plenty about why it works. Sep. 10 2011 06:20 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate AHodges from Indianapolis The placebo affect is the only reason homeopathic hucksters can make a buck. They sell a "remedy" that is so diluted that it doesn't have ANY active ingredient. Don't get taken for a ride by these quacks. Be an educated and demanding consumer! Aug. 07 2011 11:36 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate PeterAlanHOffman from wes seeya The part about the hypnosis reminds me of kungfu the legend continues where cain says something about how when the spirit is in control the body obeys. this is when he fell and hurt his hand and it healed because his mind commanded his body Jul. 11 2011 09:42 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate David from Chicago Maybe there should be a follow-up given this info: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=O1Q3jZw4FGs&feature=player_embedded Dr. Ben Goldacre does a stand-up routine about medicine, the placebo effect, and the mysteries of the human body at Nerdstock. Jan. 18 2011 02:06 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Emma This is so cool! I was so excited when I heard this just a couple of days ago. I have to do a science project for my biology class, and I didn't realize it but it was closely related to placebos! I'm entering into the science fair. Hope I get to nationals! Jan. 01 2011 02:30 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate jeffredachmd from florida Another placebo effect: A study published in JAMA reported that SSRI antidepressants are no better than placebo for most cases of depression. The authors reviewed 30 years of data and concluded that "the benefit of antidepressant medication compared with placebo may be minimal or nonexistent in patients with mild or moderate symptoms". ref JAMA 2010;303(1):47-53) For more: http://jeffreydach.com/2010/01/21/jama-says-ssri-antidepressants-are-pl acebos-by-jeffrey-dach-md.aspx jeffrey dach md Dec. 28 2010 09:45 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Josh Here is an interesting story on NPR about placebos that work even when the patient knows what they are! http://www.npr.org/blogs/health/2010/12/23/132281484/fake-pills-can-wor k-even-if-patients-know-it Dec. 23 2010 11:18 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate googie where can i find those picturs of the buy with the warts on his arms Nov. 24 2010 08:49 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate nardan from USA I hope the story about the boy with one arm healed had been checked, because in another interview it was the left arm that was healed: "So I hypnotised him and told him that the warts would go from his left arm, and sent him away." http://www.melanie-klein-trust.org.uk/masoninterview.htm Oct. 26 2010 12:21 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate mel from meadville, pa i am fairly new to radio lab, and so i will listen to the podcasts over and over to make sure i gather all of the information and interpret it well... i thought this was a wonderful "article", however i strongly disagree with the mention of the soldiers in the battle field and their way of thinking... soldiers do not think about the awards they may receive or the parades that may be had- they are trained to work as a unit and their mind set was more likely to be a thought of "someone else needs the morphine" or even that their body was naturally "high" on adrenaline with a raised level of endorphins. i relate the comradeship closely to mother and child. soldiers are willing to die for each other, much like a mother would do for her child. they bond in ways that are unimaginable. they are faced with a life and death situation and they know that the actions as a unit will win the battle, actions as an individual will lose the battle and likely the life of yourself and others. Sep. 14 2010 01:28 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate anglafro The last part of the podcast mentions an old lady and her wrist problems. In the story, the lady said that the dude massaged her wrist. Not a lot of people know that massages can temporarily cure arthritis. the old lady was tricked... Aug. 04 2010 04:42 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Bob, you said it all and effectively cut this debate short. Congrats, you earned an Honorable Mention at the International Contest of Compact Statements which Cut Discussions Short. Heh heh heh. . . Apr. 02 2010 11:54 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Bob Roney Belief may heal but ignorance kills. Mar. 20 2010 02:49 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Fast Eddie B. (Ed Benson) Just an anecdote: I saw a chiropractor for many, many years for low back problems. I would have said that the treatments seemed to work. Then I moved and stopped seeing a chiropractor. Now, 4 years later, I can say my back is better than ever, with NO manipulation. I had a similar experience with glucosamine - it seemed to work, but when I stpped taking it I could see no difference. Just my .02. Mar. 07 2010 09:16 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate dddoofus what happened to the boy whose 1 arm was cured of Congenital Ichthyosiform Erythroderma? did the rest of his skin ever heal? or just the 1 arm? find him and do a follow up interview! Jan. 26 2010 10:01 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Cindy I found it interesting that the shaman story at the beginning of the show seems to contradict the conclusion of your hypnotist doctor, yet no mention is made of this on the show. The shaman-in-training knew he was a fake, and found he healed people anyway. This seems to undercut the medical hypnotist's opinion. Jan. 13 2010 06:01 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Travis Chris, that's a terrible story, but I think you are overreacting to this broadcast. Radiolab and the people they interviewed acknowledged the limitations of the placebo effect. Would you also fault them for exploring the wonders of Asprin merely because it can't cure cancer? Jan. 08 2010 11:41 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Marco Raaphorst again such a play with audio greetings from Holland Jan. 03 2010 09:05 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Lillian Somewhat new to Radiolab, I had not heard this broadcast before. I didn't perceive this article as being about healing per se, or as espousing placebo treatments in particular, but as a conversation about what is known, and becoming more consciously known, about one of the elegant complexities of the mind. And about how we experience these phenomena daily, moment by moment, and this knowledge is reflected outright in our languages and cultures to the point that it is usually unnoticed among what we consider the conscious, logical landscape of life. Thank you, Radiolab for, again, agitating the mixture so that intriguing bubbles may surface. Dec. 31 2009 04:14 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 1/1 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate y Also, I've been going to PT and chiropractic for 3 months. Chiropractic is only half of the story. The only way to permanently change the way the spine is going to align is to strengthen it. A lot of doctors know this and will strictly not to physical therapy so that they can hold on to a patient longer. Dec. 30 2009 06:06 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate y Chris, You have a great point about pseudoscience but you can't dismiss chiropractic medicine entirely. After a violent car accident, your spine can be seriously damaged. As in, the force of the wreck can cause muscles and bones to shift in ways that cause extreme discomfort. Also, some people develop scoliosis after childhood which can cause severe pain and discomfort. Some chiropractors are all too happy to crack the bones of young children, pregnant mothers, and those of us too weak to realize that we're being ripped off. It's unfortunate that the system can be abused for money that way. On the other hand, chiropractic can be extraordinarily helpful for a large amount of people. I hurt my back at work and was told by two doctors that I probably fractured or dislocated a rib. They didn't take X-rays but pumped me full of painkillers and anti-inflammatories. I went to a chiropractor based on a recommendation of a coworker. The doctor took an X-ray and my spine was seriously messed up. About my tailbone it started to curve left, then right, then at midback it curved left again. The curve is pinching nerves in various places, leaving me with really bizarre side effects. For example, I can now tell how crooked my back is by how often I have to use the restroom. I know my bladder isn't full, but the nerves from my spine are being affected telling my bladder that it is full prematurely. Sometimes I'll go every two hours! I know it isn't related to any other disease because I am under 25 and when I go to physical therapy and chiropractic like I'm supposed to, the symptoms vanish. I haven't had trouble with that in a while since I've strengthened by back into holding its shape. Applied properly, chiropractic can provide a wealth of relief to those with GENUINE problems. Any good chiropractor should have taken X-rays regularly on a patient being treated for a crooked spine. You can't tell what is going on by touch alone. Also, if it takes 7 years to treat back pain... Perhaps 6 years ago a second opinion would have been a life saver. I don't mean any offense, I'm just saddened that these doctors sometimes put money ahead of people's lives and livelihoods. Dec. 30 2009 06:04 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Chris Smith I really am disappointed. I somehow had missed this episode in when it was originally puked out upon us. The disservice that you do is amazing. Here is another story about who the placebo effect works. A man who went to a chiropractor for 7 years straight to receive some minor relief from the constant back pain he suffered from, was informed by the new owner of the practice that perhaps he should visit with a medical doctor. He chose to do so and after three X-rays and a CT scan, the cancer diagnosis that was now unstoppable was shared with him. Of course 7 years too late; he died and left his wife with 3 boys to raise. Belief in pseudoscience kills good people. These charlatans kill people. They propagate fear, they propagate mistrust, and you have turned around and propagated that mistrust of science and medicine. How many people will now not seek real treatment because of your lack of a proper education on the science of these alleged phenomena….how many will die because of your lack of responsibility? Dec. 30 2009 04:05 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: -1/1 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Kandice Garibay This broadcast, for me, confirms the idea that thought holds more power than most people give it credit for. Just imagine how far we can go... Dec. 30 2009 03:11 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Wissam Dow Oui. I as well... Dec. 30 2009 03:16 AM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Aaron Oh, shucky darns. A rebroadcast. Oh well. I'm patiently waiting for two weeks from now. Verrrry patiently. Season 6 was great. I'm hoping for a great follow-up in season 7. Dec. 28 2009 11:44 PM Vote this comment up Vote this comment down Score: 0/0 Flag for moderation Feature this Comment Moderate Leave a Comment Register for your own account so you can vote on comments, save your favorites, and more. Learn more. Please stay on topic, be civil, and be brief. Email addresses are never displayed, but they are required to confirm your comments. Names are displayed with all comments. We reserve the right to edit any comments posted on this site. Please read the Comment Guidelines before posting. By leaving a comment, you agree to New York Public Radio's Privacy Policy and Terms Of Use. Content type* Object pk* Timestamp* Security hash* Name* ____________________ Email address* ____________________ URL ____________________ Comment* ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ If you enter anything in this field your comment will be treated as spam ____________________ Location ____________________ User name override ____________________ Site* Post Comment * Denotes a required field Podcast Support Supported by The Most * Most Viewed * Most Listened * Most Commented 1. The Times They Are a-Changin' 2. Sex, Ducks, and The Founding Feud 3. Bliss 4. Apocalyptical - Live from the Paramount in Seattle 5. Speedy Beet 6. This Is Bo, Who's Putting New Beats In New Places. You Should Meet Him 7. Weekend Special: Name That Sound! 8. Blame 9. Mutant Rights 10. What Chickadees Have That I Want. Badly 1. The Times They Are a-Changin' 2. Sex, Ducks, and The Founding Feud 3. Apocalyptical - Live from the Paramount in Seattle 4. An Ice-Cold Case 5. Cut and Run 6. Blame 7. Bliss 8. Quicksaaaand! 9. Poop Train 10. Blood 1. Weekend Special: Name That Sound! 2. The Times They Are a-Changin' 3. Blisshrooms 4. Sex, Ducks, and The Founding Feud 5. This Is Bo, Who's Putting New Beats In New Places. You Should Meet Him 6. Bliss 7. Mr. Bliss 8. Quicksaaaand! 9. What Chickadees Have That I Want. Badly 10. 23 Weeks 6 Days Newsletter Sign Up: Send me Radiolab updates Feeds Radiolab About Radiolab Radiolab is supported, in part, by the National Science Foundation and the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation, enhancing public understanding of science and technology in the modern world. More information about Sloan at www.sloan.org. WNYC * Corrections * Privacy Policy * Terms of Use * Contact * ©2014 WNYC Radio Radiolab Top * Listen * Read * Watch * Search * Support Radiolab * About § #Project Syndicate RSS Feed Friday, January 3, 2014 * Facebook * Twitter * LinkedIn * Google plus * rss Menu Back to the Homepage * Register * Sign In Back to the homepage * Home * Columnists * Sections * Syndication * About Us * Search… * ____________________ Submit See all Columnists International Economics * Michael J. Boskin * J. Bradford DeLong * Barry Eichengreen * Jeffrey Frankel * Daniel Gros * Koichi Hamada * Lee Jong-Wha * Stephen S. Roach * Kenneth Rogoff * Nouriel Roubini * Andrew Sheng * Hans-Werner Sinn * Joseph E. Stiglitz * Laura Tyson * Yu Yongding Strategic Spotlight * Shlomo Ben-Ami * Jorge G. Castañeda * Gareth Evans * Joschka Fischer * Richard N. Haass * Christopher R. Hill * Yuriko Koike * Dominique Moisi * Joseph S. Nye * Ana Palacio * Michel Rocard * Jaswant Singh * Anne-Marie Slaughter * Javier Solana * Shashi Tharoor Global Finance * Howard Davies * Mohamed A. El-Erian * Martin Feldstein * Simon Johnson * Zhang Jun * Robert J. Shiller Economics of Development * Kemal Derviş * Ricardo Hausmann * Dani Rodrik * Jeffrey D. Sachs * Michael Spence * Andrés Velasco Economic and Regulatory Policy * Jean Pisani-Ferry * Raghuram Rajan * Mark Roe Economic History * Harold James * Robert Skidelsky Public Intellectuals * Ian Buruma * Esther Dyson * Bjørn Lomborg * Peter Singer * Naomi Wolf * Economics * World Affairs * Culture & Society * Politics * Global Health & Development * Environment & Sustainability * Business & Finance * Innovation & Technology * Education Innovation & Technology Environment & Sustainability Culture & Society Photo of Ted J. Kaptchuk Ted J. Kaptchuk Ted J. Kaptchuk is Professor of Medicine at Harvard Medical School and Director of the Program in Placebo Studies at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center in Boston. read more Photo of John M. Kelley John M. Kelley John M. Kelley, a professor of psychology at Endicott College and an instructor at Harvard Medical School, is Deputy Director of the Program in Placebo Studies at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center in Boston. read more Exit from comment view mode. Click to hide this space * English + Arabic + Chinese + French + German + Spanish * Mail to friendMail to friend * PrintPrint OCT 18, 2013 1 Inside the Placebo Effect * * * * * * BOSTON – For many medical researchers and followers of science, few things are more unsettling than the placebo effect. How can an inert sugar pill have therapeutic value? The answer requires understanding the context that surrounds medical treatment – a setting in which the symbols and rituals of health care combine with the charged emotional reactions that arise when patients encounter healers. The importance of trust, empathy, hope, fear, trepidation, and uncertainty in the therapeutic encounter should not be disregarded. By using sugar pills, saline injections, or even sham surgery, placebo research isolates provision of care from the direct effects of genuine medications or procedures. Recent research on the placebo effect has demonstrated that the clinical encounter alone – without the provision of any “real” medicine – can alleviate pain, improve sleep, relieve depression, and ameliorate the symptoms of a wide variety of conditions, including irritable bowel syndrome, asthma, Parkinson’s disease, heart ailments, and migraine. Placebos mainly influence patient self-appraisal. They cannot shrink tumors; but they can help patients experience less of the fatigue, nausea, pain, and anxiety that are associated with cancer and its treatment. They cannot lower cholesterol or reduce high blood pressure, but they may alter mood or pain sufficiently to promote more healthful behaviors. Placebos can behave like drugs; and the placebo effect can also make drugs more effective. Research shows that various components of the placebo effect – for example, the paraphernalia of care (pills and syringes) and the patient-provider relationship – can be added incrementally in a manner analogous to dose dependence (the higher the dose, the greater the effect). Indeed, these components have been shown to boost the efficacy of many powerful medications. For example, when morphine is administered by injection in full view of the patient, it is significantly stronger than when it is given through an intravenous line without the patient’s knowledge. Many psychosocial mechanisms have been implicated in placebo responses. Increased hope, positive expectations, and reduced anxiety can all modify “mindsets” that guide how patients respond to noxious sensations. Evidence strongly suggests that the support and empathy of a thoughtful and attentive physician can improve clinical outcomes. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that non-conscious environmental cues and symbols – the white coat or the diploma on the wall – can “prime” a patient to experience improvement. Until recently, it was assumed that the effects of placebo pills depended on concealment or deception. The patient had to believe that the treatment was “real” for placebos to work. But new research indicates the potential for significant clinical improvement even if patients are told that they are ingesting an inactive substance. This suggests that the simple enactment of a treatment ritual may, like conscious expectations, have a powerful impact. In fact, the power of imagination, it seems, has a basis in neurobiology. Recent evidence shows that when placebos have salubrious effects, they engage the same neurological pathways as active medications. For example, when patients experience pain relief from placebos, the brain releases endogenous opioids and/or CB1 cannabinoids – the very same mechanisms that mediate pain relief derived from pharmaceutical treatments. Likewise, neuroimaging studies show that placebo treatment activates specific brain structures such as the prefrontal cortex and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex. Experiments on patients with Parkinson’s disease have shown that placebo treatment releases endogenous dopamine in the striatum region of the brain. Moreover, intriguing pilot research suggests that there may be genetic factors that predispose one to have greater placebo responses. The effects of placebos are not always beneficial. The placebo effect has a dark twin called the nocebo effect. Although placebos are biologically inert, as many as 26% of placebo-treated patients drop out of clinical trials after suffering intolerable side effects, which are usually the same as the possible side effects of the drug being tested. For example, in a trial of anti-migraine medication, if the active ingredient is an anticonvulsant, the nocebo effect (the placebo’s side effect) will disproportionally relate to anorexia or memory; but if the active ingredient is a non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug, the nocebo effect will more likely be gastrointestinal symptoms and thirst. This underscores the importance of placebo effects in the development of new drugs. To approve new pharmaceuticals, the US Food and Drug Administration requires two well-designed randomized controlled trials in which the drug demonstrates superiority over placebo treatment. However, evidence suggests that for some illnesses, placebo effects have grown progressively larger over the last several decades. This “placebo drift” poses significant challenges to detecting drug-placebo differences. And that highlights a more fundamental point: In our rush to embrace high-tech medicine, we tend to forget the enormous potential for healing that can arise from a good therapeutic relationship. Placebo research has demonstrated that the context within which treatment takes place and the patient-clinician relationship have huge potential to improve health outcomes. We need to learn more about the placebo effect’s power and limitations. We also need to learn how to translate this scientific knowledge into ethical and effective methods that physicians can use to improve medical outcomes. And we need to know more about the placebo effect in clinical trials. In short, we need to stop thinking in terms of the “art of medicine” and start exploring a new science of healing. * Contact us to secure rights * Previous Winning the Fight Against Killer Diseases Jeffrey D. Sachs * Next Measure for Mismeasure Esther Dyson Exit from comment view mode. Click to hide this space Hide Comments Hide Comments Read Comments (1) Please login or register to post a comment * Sign in * Register * [X] Experts * [X] Users 1. + Reply + Vote down + Vote up CommentedZsolt Hermann NOV 7, 2013 Thank you for this intriguing article. I agree with everything the article describes, as a surgeon myself, and I am sure other health professionals can testify "intrinsic" factors are crucial, we could even say most important in healing, acquiring illness and health in general. I would suggest that while modern medicine looks at illness and healing as an external process, illness happening due to external causes, pathogens, environmental factors, and healing also have to come "from the outside" from overwhelming pharmaceutical agents, improving technology and surgical miracles and of course expensive implants, true illness/healing is a completely internal matter. Although Indian or Chinese traditional medicine has several thousands of years experience with this viewpoint, by today even in their original countries very few people remember, let alone practice this notion. All medicine should be doing is facilitating, initiating a healthy protective, healing response from the body and the body then heals itself. And beyond medicine the whole human environment should function in such natural, self supporting way as any other interconnected natural, living system providing healthy, sustainable living, strong inner protection to all the individuals and the whole. Featured Photo of George Soros EconomicsJAN 2, 2014 5 The World Economy’s Shifting Challenges George Soros maps the terrain of a global economy that is increasingly shaped by China. Photo of J. Bradford DeLong EconomicsDEC 31, 2013 15 The Strange Case of American Inequality J. Bradford DeLong asks why Americans are not clamoring for polices that would leave 90% of them better off. Photo of Naomi Wolf Business & FinanceDEC 31, 2013 2 The Girl Can’t Help It Naomi Wolf decodes the sexist media coverage of Mary Barra's appointment as CEO of GM. * * * * * * * Mail to friendMail to friend * PrintPrint Menu back to home * Columnists * Sections + Economics + World Affairs + Culture & Society + Politics + Global Health & Development + Environment & Sustainability + Business & Finance + Innovation & Technology + Education * Blogs * Syndication * About * ____________________ Submit * Register * Sign In Quantcast Email this Article Your Name ____________________ Your Email ____________________ Friend’s Name ____________________ Friend’s Email ____________________ Message ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ Incorrect please try again Enter the words above Enter the numbers you hear ____________________ Captcha is not valid Get another captcha Get an audio captcha Get an image captcha Help Thank you. You're human. IFRAME: http://www.google.com/recaptcha/api/noscript?k=6LfJWcoSAAAAALdWp9SKKlds tk6cxXJEqzzrnFjB _____________________________________ ________________________________________ ________________________________________ Send Cancel § #alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate alternate Urban Word of the Day Urban Dictionary Search Subscribe Feedback English * Arabic * Azerbaijani * Bengali * Bulgarian * Chinese * Czech * Danish * Dutch * English * Filipino * French * German * Greek * Hebrew * Hungarian * Indonesian * Italian * Japanese * Korean * Norwegian * Polish * Portuguese * Romanian * Russian * Serbian * Spanish * Swedish * Thai * Turkish * Ukrainian * Vietnamese * * Help translate! look up any word, like thot: placebo effect______ search word of the day word of the year favorites dictionary game thesaurus names media store add blog ____________________ random A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z # new tv [INS: :INS] trending * smegma * poopsterbate * tittybong * thot * totes magotes * king hit * broken ankles * sounding * bae * flurge categories * gaming * sports * food * sex * tv * film * celebrities * military * music * weather * insults see also * placebo alphabetical * placate * Placation * Placcy * Place * Placeball * placebic * Placeblow * placebo * Place bo * placebo button * placebocism * Placebo Day * Placebo Degree * placebo drift * Placebo Drink * placebo effect * Placebo Fart * placebo friend * Placebo Hangover * placeboing * Placeboism * Placebo Love * placebooze * Placebos * placebo status * placebo surgery * Placebo Syndrome * Placebot * placeboware * placebro * Placebro effect * Placebro (pla- see- bro) * placebuzz * placed * placedrop * place drop * Placeholder * Place-holder * placeholder boyfriend * Placeholder girlfriend * placeist 1. placebo effect This is mind over matter; when somebody believes that something will happen to his health, it does even when it wouldn't have otherwise. This generally happens when doctors tell patients that their health will improve even though there's no logical reason to believe that; the patients' optimism then helps heal them. The doctor gave Jimmy sugar capsules and told them it would cure his headaches; it did even though the capsules didn't do anything. Classic placebo effect! mark as favorite buy placebo effect mugs & shirts mind over matter illogical doctor medicine psychology by iLikeSoup February 17, 2010 add a video add an image Random Word 2. placebo effect When you are with people who are doing something, even when you arent doing it, you feel the same effect. It works depending on if you mentally believe in it or not. Since I'm straight edge, when my friends drink, i use the placebo effect and feel the same as they do. mark as favorite buy placebo effect mugs & shirts straight edge triple x bud bong piece by taaaaayyyyllooorrrr January 11, 2009 add a video add an image advertise here [INS: :INS] ©1999-2014 Urban Dictionary® terms of service privacy feedback remove advertise chat technology jobs api rss and gcal facebook sms twitter (BUTTON) × Free Daily Email Type your email address below to get our free Urban Word of the Day every morning! ____________________ Subscribe Emails are sent from daily@urbandictionary.com. We'll never spam you. (BUTTON) × My Favorites Log in with Facebook to save your favorite definitions and share them with friends. (BUTTON) Login with Facebook (BUTTON) × Customer support is only available in English. Quantcast § HEALTHbeat Harvard's FREE E-mail Newsletter In each issue of HEALTHbeat: * Get trusted advice from the doctors at Harvard Medical School * Learn tips for living a healthy lifestyle * Stay up-to-date on the latest developments in health * Receive special offers on health books and reports * Plus, receive your FREE Bonus Report, Living to 100: What's the secret? Sign up today and get 25% off your first purchase. E-mail Address ________________________ First Name (Optional) ________________________ Sign Up Now [ Maybe Later ] [ No Thanks ] Harvard Health Publications [three_doctors.png] * MY ACCOUNT * MY LIBRARY * DICTIONARY * HELP Customer Sign In Shopping Cart (0 items for $0.00) Enter a Keyword to Searc [button-white_arrow.gif]-Submit * Home * Health Newsletters * Special Health Reports * Health Books * Browse By Topic * Blog Text size: AAA Check out these newly released Special Health Reports from Harvard Medical School Learn How New Releases You can't buy good health but you can buy good health information. Check out these newly released Special Health Reports from Harvard Medical School: * Living Wills * Boosting Your Energy * Stress Management * Core Exercises Harvard Health Letter: April 2012 April 2012 SINGLE ISSUE (*) Online Access (PDF) $5.00 [icon-help.gif] Add To Cart Browse Additional Issues << >> * 2006 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2007 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2008 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2009 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2010 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2011 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2012 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2013 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC * 2014 JAN FEB MAR APR MAY JUN JUL AUG SEP OCT NOV DEC Articles In the April 2012 Issue of Harvard Health Letter * Putting the placebo effect to work * In Brief: Vigorous exercise produces 'afterburn' bonus * In Brief: Tai chi helps Parkinson's patients with balance, movement * 6 ways to tame the modern muffin * Update on cataract surgery and replacement lenses * Ask the doctor: Should I be worried about the side effects from cortisone shots? * Ask the doctor: Is Vaseline a good face cream? « View the Issue Overview Health Resources * Current and Archived News * Family Health Guide Online * Guide to Diagnostic Tests * HEALTHbeat Archive * Medical Dictionary * Medical Symptom Checker * Newsweek Readers HEALTHbeat HEALTHbeat Harvard's FREE E-Newsletter Get health information and advice from the experts at Harvard Medical School. Sign up today and get 25% off your first purchase. Enter E-mail Address____ Enter First Name (Option Sign Up Now Home » Newsletters » Harvard Health Letter » April 2012 » Putting the placebo effect to work Putting the placebo effect to work APR 2012 Did you know? You can get instant online access to all of the articles from the April 2012 issue of Harvard Health Letter for only $5. Add To Cart Already a subscriber? Login for complete instant access. If you want a button/link to remove the box (not sure if you do or not...), it would look like this: Cancel Rather than dismiss it, we should try to understand the placebo effect and harness it when we can. For a long time, the placebo effect was held in low regard. If people responded to a suspect treatment, we said it was "just the placebo effect." The suggestion was that they had been fooled in some way, and their response was inauthentic. But attitudes are shifting, even in conventional medical circles. Randomized trials, some of them led by researchers at the Harvard-wide Program in Placebo Studies and the Therapeutic Encounter, have deepened the understanding of the placebo effect and its various components. Researchers have also used brain scans and other technologies to show that there may be a physiological explanation for the placebo effect in many cases. There is some danger that uncritical acceptance of the placebo effect could be used to justify useless treatments. But more important is the growing recognition that what we call the placebo effect may involve changes in brain chemistry — and that the placebo effect may be an integral part of good medical care and an ally that should be embraced by doctors and patients alike. The power of expectation Arriving at a tidy definition of the placebo effect is difficult, but here's a try: it's a favorable response to a medical intervention — a pill, a procedure, a counseling session, you name it — that doesn't have a direct physiological effect. (Note the emphasis on direct, because there do seem to be indirect effects.) The classic example is when people enrolled in a study experience some improvement in their condition even though they have been assigned, for comparison purposes, to take "dummy" pills that don't contain any active ingredients. Expectations appear to have a lot to do with the effect. If an intervention is believed to help a condition, a certain percentage of people who receive it will experience some benefit. How large a percentage varies tremendously and depends on the condition, the strength of belief, the subjectivity of the response, and many other factors. The placebo effect may also have an element of psychological conditioning: once someone benefits from an intervention, the person starts to associate that intervention with a benefit. The association, and therefore the benefit, may get stronger with additional exposures to the intervention. An effect of care that's caring There's also evidence that some of the placebo effect is a favorable reaction to care and attention from people who patients believe can help ease their suffering and distress. Researchers associated with Harvard's placebo studies program published a study in 2008 that illustrates this aspect of the response very nicely. The volunteers for the study were people with irritable bowel syndrome (IBS), a condition that causes abdominal pain and changes in bowel movements in the absence of any discernible changes to the bowel. The placebo treatment was sham acupuncture, which involves the use of needles that, unbeknownst to the patient, retract into their handles instead of penetrating the skin. The placebo effect of the sham acupuncture needles was impressive: 44% of those treated with just the sham needles reported relief from their IBS problems. When sham acupuncture was combined with attentive, empathetic interaction with the acupuncturist, the placebo effect got even larger, with 62% reporting relief from their IBS woes. Subjective vs. objective The placebo effect is most pronounced and relevant when a treatment's success or failure depends largely on the subjective experiences of patients. That's often the case for conditions that are defined mainly by symptoms, such as depression, and problems like migraine headaches and back pain that are defined primarily by the pain they produce. Sometimes treatments can be judged by both objective and subjective outcomes, and a Harvard study published in 2011 in The New England Journal of Medicine showed that the placebo effect may influence one and not the other. This small study compared the response of 39 people with asthma to albuterol, an inhaled bronchodilator that's commonly used to treat asthma, with two different inactive treatments, a placebo inhaler and sham acupuncture. Albuterol was much more effective than both the placebo inhaler and the sham acupuncture when an objective test of lung function, called FEV1, was used to measure the response of the asthmatic study volunteers. But when the study volunteers were asked about their symptoms and perceived improvement in their asthma, albuterol and the placebos performed equally well. Research is showing that the placebo effect often seems to be associated with objective changes in brain chemistry. A number of studies have shown, for example, that the brain releases natural pain-relieving substances, called endorphins, when people enrolled in pain studies are given placebos. Research results indicate that measurable changes in brain chemistry may explain the large placebo effect seen in depression treatment. Parkinson's disease is associated with a shortage of a brain chemical called dopamine, and in studies of the disease, placebos have increased the production of dopamine. Getting it out in the open In 2008, just under 700 American internists and rheumatologists were surveyed about prescribing placebos. Only a small percentage had ever prescribed "pure" placebos like sugar pills or saline solutions. But about 40% reported that they had prescribed over-the-counter pain relievers or vitamins as placebos, not because they believed these compounds would have a direct effect on the person's condition. Even if patients feel better, prescribing pain relievers and vitamins in that way involves some deception. Dr. Ted Kaptchuk, director of the Harvard placebo program, and colleagues have conducted "open-label" placebo studies, in which patients were told they were taking a placebo and that the placebo effect is powerful. In one small study of IBS patients, Dr. Kaptchuk and his colleagues found that an open-label placebo still produced a placebo effect and was about 20% more effective than no treatment. These and other results suggest that doctors could be more up-front with patients, informing them that a prescription is mainly for placebo purposes, and their patients would still benefit, as long as the prescription had no major drawbacks. What you can do We're a long way from fully understanding the placebo effect. But here are some things you can do (and think), based on what researchers have discovered so far: Make sure you're getting the support you need from your doctor. Placebo effect research has shown how important a supportive doctor-patient relationship can be. If you're not getting the support and attention you need, consider switching doctors. Recognize that it might be "in your head" — but there's nothing wrong with that. Behind the subjective experience of feeling better (and worse) are objective changes in brain chemistry that we've only started to understand. Find treatments you can believe in… Expectations that an intervention will have some benefit increase the chances that it will. …but keep your healthy skepticism. Quacks and charlatans can exploit the placebo effect to peddle treatments that are useless, and even harmful, if for no other reason than they keep people from getting treatment that is directly effective. Tweet Share Print Harvard Health Publications * About Us * Our Blog * Privacy Policy * Shipping Policy Follow us on: Twitter Facebook Contact Us * Customer Service * Change address + The Harvard Health Letter + The Harvard Heart Letter + The Harvard Women's Health Watch + The Harvard Men's Health Watch * Pay bill + The Harvard Health Letter + The Harvard Heart Letter + The Harvard Women's Health Watch + The Harvard Men's Health Watch * Renew subscription + The Harvard Health Letter + The Harvard Heart Letter + The Harvard Women's Health Watch + The Harvard Men's Health Watch * Check expiration date + The Harvard Health Letter + The Harvard Heart Letter + The Harvard Women's Health Watch + The Harvard Men's Health Watch * Order a gift subscription + The Harvard Health Letter + The Harvard Heart Letter + The Harvard Women's Health Watch + The Harvard Men’s Health Watch * Missing Issues + The Harvard Health Letter + The Harvard Heart Letter + The Harvard Women's Health Watch + The Harvard Men's Health Watch * Media Inquiries * Technical Support * Help E-mail The Editor * Special Health Reports * Harvard Health Letter * Harvard Women's Health Watch * Harvard Men's Health Watch * Harvard Heart Letter Corporate Sales * Permissions * Licensing * Bulk Sales © 2000-2014 Harvard University. All rights reserved. Site Map [icon-healthbeat.gif] HEALTHbeat Harvard's FREE E-Newsletter. Sign up today and get 25% off your first purchase. Enter E-mail Address____ Enter First Name (Option Sign Up Now Quantcast § 0 Menu * Home * Current Issue * All Issues * Online First * Collections 19 March 2002, Vol 136, No. 6 < Previous in this Issue | Next in this Issue > * PDF Ideas and Opinions Published online 19 March 2002 Deconstructing the Placebo Effect and Finding the Meaning Response Daniel E. Moerman, PhD; Wayne B. Jonas, MD [+-] Author Affiliations From University of Michigan, Dearborn, Michigan; and Samueli Institute and Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, Bethesda, Maryland. Grant Support: In part by a grant from the National Science Foundation to Dr. Moerman (NSF SBR-9421128). Requests for Single Reprints: Daniel E. Moerman, PhD, Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan–Dearborn, 6515 Cherry Hill Road, Ypsilanti, MI 48198; e-mail, dmoerman@umich.edu. Current Author Addresses: Dr. Moerman: Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan–Dearborn, 6515 Cherry Hill Road, Ypsilanti, MI 48198. Dr. Jonas: Department of Family Medicine, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, Bethesda MD 20814. More Author Information Ann Intern Med. 19 March 2002,136(6):471-476 doi:10.7326/0003-4819-136-6-200203190-00011 [+-] More Article Comments (0) We provide a new perspective with which to understand what for a half century has been known as the “placebo effect.” We argue that, as currently used, the concept includes much that has nothing to do with placebos, confusing the most interesting and important aspects of the phenomenon. We propose a new way to understand those aspects of medical care, plus a broad range of additional human experiences, by focusing on the idea of “meaning,” to which people, when they are sick, often respond. We review several of the many areas in medicine in which meaning affects illness or healing and introduce the idea of the “meaning response.” We suggest that use of this formulation, rather than the fixation on inert placebos, will probably lead to far greater insight into how treatment works and perhaps to real improvements in human well-being. Topics placebo effect Your Session has timed out. Please sign back in to continue. Sign In to Access Full Content Sign In * Sign in as individual > * Sign in as institution > * Forgot your password? Forgot Your Password? Learn how to get mobile access Figures Tables Interactive Graphics Video Letter __________________________________________________________________ Loading... Loading Optional Evaluation for CME course: CME Course for: To understand the clinical management of acute heart failure syndromes. THIS PAGE IS REQUIRED READING BEFORE BEGINNING ALL ACCME-ACCREDITED COURSES ACCREDITATION STATEMENT The American College of Physicians is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education (ACCME) to provide continuing education for physicians. The American College of Physicians designates this journal-based CME Activity, Annals articles, for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 Credit™. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. The American College of Physicians designates this Enduring Material, In the Clinic, for a maximum of 1.5 AMA PRA Category 1 Credits™. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. The American College of Physicians designates this Enduring Material, ACP Ethics Manual, Sixth Edition, for a maximum of 10 AMA PRA Category 1 Credits™. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. The American College of Physicians designates this enduring material, Consult Guys, for a maximum of .5 AMA PRA Category 1 Credits™. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. To claim CME credit participants will view the appropriate content and successfully complete a CME quiz with a score of 100%. MISSION STATEMENT/PROGRAM OBJECTIVES After evaluating specific content published by Annals of Internal Medicine, participants in the CME activity should be able to demonstrate an increase in, or affirmation of, their knowledge of clinical medicine. Participants should be able to evaluate the appropriateness of the clinical information as it applies to the provision of patient care. PARTICIPANTS This program is designed for physicians who are involved in providing patient care and who wish to advance their current knowledge of clinical medicine. HARDWARE/BROWSER REQUIREMENTS Find out more about minimum hardware and browser requirements for use with this CME activity, in the FAQ section. PRIVACY STATEMENT Annals of Internal Medicine CME faculty have established a Privacy Statement for our organization and users. DISCLOSURES Christine Laine, MD, MPH, Editor in Chief, reports that she has no financial relationships or interests to disclose. Darren B. Taichman, MD, PhD, Executive Deputy Editor, reports that he has received research support from a grant to the University of Pennsylvania from Actelion. Cynthia D. Mulrow, MD, MSc, Senior Deputy Editor, reports that she has no financial relationships or interests to disclose. Deborah Cotton, MD, MPH, Deputy Editor, reports that she has no financial relationships or interests to disclose. Jaya K. Rao, MD, MHS, reports that she has stock options/holdings in Eli Lilly and Pfizer. Sankey V. Williams, MD, Deputy Editor, reports that he has no financial relationships or interests to disclose. Catharine B. Stack, PhD, MS, Deputy Editor, Statistics, reports that she has stock holdings in Pfizer. For the Consult Guys: Geno J. Merli, MD, MACP, FHM, FSVM disclosures are as follows: Research Grants/Contracts: Bristol-Myers Squibb, Bayer, Sanofi-aventis. Consultantship: Bristol-Myers Squibb, Bayer, Sanofi-aventis COMMERCIAL SUPPORT This CME program receives no commercial support. Submit Note: You must get of the correct answers to pass this quiz. Note: You must get of the correct answers to pass this quiz. SUBMIT ANSWERS CLEAR ANSWERS SAVE FOR LATER RETURN TO QUIZ You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz The following questions were not answered: Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of The following questions were not answered correctly: Return to Quiz Your quiz results: The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted SUBMIT EVALUATION CANCEL NOTE: Citing articles are presented as examples only. In non-demo SCM6 implementation, integration with CrossRef’s "Cited By" API will populate this tab (http://www.crossref.org/citedby.html). Compression-Only CPR: Pushing the Science Forward Cone AAM 2010;304:1493-1495. All you need to read in the other general jounals BMJ 2010;341:c5893-c1494. Please read the other comments before posting. Contributors must reveal any conflict of interest. Comments are moderated and will appear on the site at the discretion of The American College of Physicians editorial staff. Please be sure your email address is updated in your account, otherwise the American College of Physicians will not be able to contact you about your comment. * = Required Field Comment Author(s)* (if multiple authors, separate names by comma) ____________________ Example: John Doe Affiliation & Institution* ____________________ Disclosure of Any Conflicts of Interest* (applies to the past 5 years and foreseeable future) Indicate any potential conflicts of interest of each author below, including specific financial interests and relationships and affiliations relevant to the subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript (eg, employment/affiliation, grants or funding, consultancies, honoraria, speakers bureau, stock ownership or options, expert testimony, royalties, donation of medical equipment, or patents filed, received, or pending). If all authors have none, check "No potential conflicts or relevant financial interests" in the box below. Please also indicate any funding received in support of this work. The information will be posted with your response. _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ Please disclose any conflicts of interest or check the box below. [X] No potential conflicts or relevant financial interests Comment Title* ____________________ Comment* _____ _____ _____ _____ _____ Get Audio Code ____________________ Please Wait... Submit Cancel Submit a Comment Submit a Comment [b_download.png] Submit a Letter [b_download.png] Submit a Comment Sign In Sign In * Sign in as individual > * Sign in as institution > * Forgot your password? Forgot Your Password? Learn how to get mobile access SUBSCRIPTION UPSELL Article Purchase and Publication Subscription Area Related Articles The Placebo Effect in Alternative Medicine: Can the Performance of a Healing Ritual Have Clinical Significance? Ann Intern Med. 2002;136(11):817-825. The Doctor-Patient Relationship Revisited: An Analysis of the Placebo Effect Ann Intern Med. 1973;78(4):595-598. [+] View More Topic Collections * Cardiology * Gastroenterology/Hepatology * Hematology/Oncology * Neurology * Ethics * Headache * Peptic Disease * Peptic Ulcer * Coronary Heart Disease You do not have access to this content. You either do not have a subscription or your subscription has expired. Click here to Renew Now ____________________ * Home * Online First * Current Issue * Collections * All Issues Sign In © American College of Physicians. All Rights Reserved. § [p?c1=2&c2=16939915&cv=2.0&cj=1] ScienceDaily: Your source for the latest research news and science breakthroughs -- updated daily Science News ... from universities, journals, and other research organizations Save Email Print Share Placebo Effect and Lessons for the Physician-Patient Relationship Sep. 19, 2013 — The findings of a comprehensive review of the placebo phenomenon and its consequences for clinical medicine are contained in a new article, "Placebo and the New Physiology of the Doctor-Patient Relationship," published in Physiological Reviews. The effort, undertaken by physician-researcher Fabrizio Benedetti of the Department of Neuroscience, University of Turin Medical School, and National Institute of Neuroscience, Turin, Italy, provides an in-depth biological and evolutionary approach to examining the placebo effect in relationship to the doctor-patient relationship. __________________________________________________________________ Share This: Placebo, in Latin, means "I shall please," and its role in research and medicine is a fascinating story. One of the first recorded use of placebos involved Benjamin Franklin who was commissioned by the French king Louis XVI to test the effectiveness of mesmerism, a kind of healing practice which was supposed to act through a healing fluid released from the healer. Franklin's team utilized blind assessments and placebo interventions to women patients and determined that their improvements were spurred on by the imagination. Over time there was awareness to both researchers and physicians that clinical trials were susceptible to imagination and biases. This led to the use of the double-blind design, in which neither the investigator nor the patient knew the nature of the tested therapy (it could be either real or fake). For many years placebos have been used for the validation of therapies, but they have also traditionally taken as an example of the powerful interaction between mind and body with associated commentary research appearing in psychology literature. Dr. Benedetti's research is aligned with the current state of placebo research, a complex field of investigation which ranges from psychology to psychophysiology, from pharmacology to neurophysiology, and from cellular/molecular analysis to modern neuroimaging techniques. Dr. Benedetti's research methodology employed in this article transcends the traditional division between psychology, the study of the mind and how it works, and biology, the study of all living things. This article clarifies the research conducted to identify the relationship between pharmacological treatments administered to the patient and the role of the mind in the overall patient health. In addition, it clarifies the interaction between psychological processes and the many physiological functions of the human body. This new discipline acknowledges that placebos and placebo responses with their wide range of physiological responses involving numerous mechanisms across a number of conditions, systems, and interventions represent an active field of neurobiological research. With that, Dr. Benedetti, using biochemical, cellular and physiological tools, aptly summarizes research new findings on describing the placebo effect on psychology and biology and their impact on the doctor-patient relationship. Among the issues discussed in detail are: o There is no one "the placebo effect." There are different mechanisms in play across a variety of medical conditions and therapeutic interventions. For example, a placebo effect takes place because there is expectation. The patient expects a therapeutic benefit, and this kind of expectation actually has an effect on the brain and the body. o The connection between expectation and real improvement that may occur is due at least to two mechanisms. The first may be a reduction of anxiety. The second is between expectation and the activation of a reward mechanism by the region of the brain known as the nucleus accumbens (which also governs pleasure, reinforcement learning, laughter, addiction, aggression, fear, impulsivity and the placebo effect.) o When a treatment is given to a patient, be it a placebo or real, it is administered in a complex set of psychological states that vary from patient to patient and from situation to situation. For example, when a placebo is given to relieve pain, it is administered along with stimuli which tell the patient that a clinical improvement should be occurring shortly. These stimuli can include the color and shape of the pill, patient and provider characteristics and the healthcare setting. o Recent research has revealed a reduced efficacy of drugs when they are administered covertly to the patient. In fact, if the placebo/expectation component of a treatment is eliminated by means of a hidden administration (unbeknownst to the patient), the psychological component of the therapy is absent as well. For physicians, psychologists, and health professionals these and other recent findings found in Dr. Benedetti's article can foster enhanced understanding of how their words, attitudes, and behaviors impact on the physiological profile of their patients' brains. This "direct vision" of the patient's brain will hopefully boost health professionals' empathic, humane, and compassionate behavior further. Moreover, understanding the physiological underpinnings of the doctor-patient relationship will lead to better medical practice as well as to better social/communication skills and health policy. Share this story on Facebook, Twitter, and Google: Other social bookmarking and sharing tools: | __________________________________________________________________ Story Source: The above story is based on materials provided by American Physiological Society (APS). Note: Materials may be edited for content and length. For further information, please contact the source cited above. __________________________________________________________________ Journal Reference: 1. Fabrizio Benedetti. PLACEBO AND THE NEW PHYSIOLOGY OF THE DOCTOR-PATIENT RELATIONSHIP. Physiological Reviews, September 2013 DOI: 10.%u200B1152/%u200Bphysrev.%u200B00043.%u200B2012 Need to cite this story in your essay, paper, or report? Use one of the following formats: (*) APA ( ) MLA Note: If no author is given, the source is cited instead. Related Topics __________________________________________________________________ Health & Medicine * Patient Education and Counseling * Today's Healthcare * Personalized Medicine Mind & Brain * Disorders and Syndromes * Psychology * Social Psychology Articles * Double blind * Placebo effect * Clinical trial * Evidence-based medicine * Nocebo - Placebo * List of medical topics Related Stories __________________________________________________________________ Intranasal Application of Oxytocin Appears to Enhance Placebo Response (Oct. 22, 2013) — A study has been conducted to test whether oxytocin enhances the placebo response in an experimental placebo analgesia ... > read more Doctor-Patient Relationship: Physicians' Brain Scans Indicate Doctors Can Feel Their Patients' Pain -- And Their Relief (Jan. 29, 2013) — In a novel investigation in which physicians underwent brain scans while they believed they were actually treating patients, researchers have provided the first scientific evidence indicating that ... > read more Nocebo Effect, Not Placebo Effect: Induced Illness Studied (July 12, 2012) — Negative suggestion can induce symptoms of illness. Nocebo effects are the adverse events that occur during sham treatment and/or as a result of negative expectations. While the positive counterpart ... > read more Placebo Effects In Caregivers May Change Behavior Of Children With ADHD (June 30, 2009) — Stimulant medications, such as Ritalin and Adderall, are the accepted treatment to stem hyperactivity in children with attention deficit-hyperactive disorder and improve their behavior. Now a recent ... > read more Pain Relieving Effects Of Acupuncture Are Limited (Jan. 29, 2009) — The pain relieving effects of acupuncture compared with placebo are small and seem to lack clinical relevance, according to a study published on the British Medical Journal ... > read more Search ScienceDaily Number of stories in archives: 149,309 __________________________________________________________________ Find with keyword(s): ______________________________________ Search Enter a keyword or phrase to search ScienceDaily's archives for related news topics, the latest news stories, reference articles, science videos, images, and books. __________________________________________________________________ Recommend ScienceDaily on Facebook, Twitter, and Google: Other social bookmarking and sharing services: | Coming Soon: The New ScienceDaily Preview our new website, featuring all the latest research news in an updated, streamlined design, with videos from news services around the world. Just In: Odor Receptors Discovered in Lungs Antarctic Glacier Sensitive To Climate Change Strong Boost to Binary-Star Formation Theory Hubble Sees Cloudy Weather On Alien World Solution to Cloud Riddle Reveals Hotter Future How Emotions Are Mapped in the Body Major Reductions in Marine Life Predicted Earth's Crust Dripped to Mantle in Archean Eon more breaking science news Social Networks __________________________________________________________________ Follow ScienceDaily on Facebook, Twitter, and Google: Facebook Twitter Google+ Recommend ScienceDaily on Facebook, Twitter, and Google +1: Other social bookmarking and sharing tools: | Breaking News ... from NewsDaily.com __________________________________________________________________ more science news In Other News ... more top news Science Video News __________________________________________________________________ Surviving Heart Failure Inflammations that occur after a heart attack can be a severe complication that further damages the heart. Cardiologists are now trying a new. ... > full story * Bioengineers Combine Mathematical Equations, Data to Simulate Surgeries * Information Technologists Design System To Recognize Palm Vein Patterns * New Cast Skate Absorbs Impact, Distributes Weight, and Protects the Cast * more science videos Strange Science News __________________________________________________________________ * Simulator Recreates Virtual Taste Online * No Scrounging for Scraps: Research Uncovers Diets of Middle, Lower Class in Pompeii * Money Talks When Ancient Antioch Meets Google Earth * How Emotions Are Mapped in the Body * Ancient Traditions: Why We Make New Year Resolutions * more strange science news Free Subscriptions ... from ScienceDaily __________________________________________________________________ Get the latest science news with our free email newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds in your RSS reader: * Email Newsletters * RSS Newsfeeds Feedback ... we want to hear from you! __________________________________________________________________ Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions? * Leave Feedback __________________________________________________________________ Save Email Print Share About ScienceDaily^(R) | Editorial Staff | Awards & Reviews | Contribute News | Advertise With Us | Contact Us | Privacy Policy | Terms of Use Copyright 2013 by ScienceDaily, LLC or by third-party sources, where indicated. All rights controlled by their respective owners. Content on this website is for informational purposes only. It is not intended to provide medical or other professional advice. Views expressed on this website do not necessarily reflect those of ScienceDaily, its staff, or its partners. * Health & Medicine + Allergy + Alternative Medicine + Birth Control + Cancer + Diabetes + Diseases and Conditions + Heart Disease + HIV and AIDS + Obesity + Stem Cells + ... more topics * Mind & Brain + ADD and ADHD + Addiction + Alzheimer's + Autism + Depression + Headaches + Intelligence + Perception + Psychology + Relationships + Schizophrenia + ... more topics * Plants & Animals + Agriculture and Food + Animals + Behavior + Biology + Biotechnology + Endangered Animals + Extinction + Genetically Modified + Microbes and More + New Species + Zoology + ... more topics * Earth & Climate + Climate + Earthquakes + Environment + Geography + Geology + Global Warming + Hurricanes + Ozone Holes + Pollution + Weather + ... more topics * Space & Time + Astronomy + Black Holes + Dark Matter + Extrasolar Planets + Mars + Moon + Solar System + Space Telescopes + Stars + Sun + ... more topics * Matter & Energy + Aviation + Chemistry + Electronics + Fossil Fuels + Nanotechnology + Physics + Quantum Physics + Solar Energy + Technology + Wind Energy + ... more topics * Computers & Math + Artificial Intelligence + Communications + Computer Science + Hacking + Mathematics + Quantum Computers + Robotics + Software + Video Games + Virtual Reality + ... more topics * Fossils & Ruins + Ancient Civilizations + Anthropology + Archaeology + Dinosaurs + Early Humans + Early Mammals + Evolution + Lost Treasures + Origin of Life + Paleontology + ... more topics Search ...__________ Go * News + Front Page + Today's Summaries + Latest Headlines + Top News + Search Archives + Living Well + Strange Science + Most Popular + RSS Newsfeeds + Email Newsletters + News Sections + Health & Medicine + Mind & Brain + Plants & Animals + Space & Time + Earth & Climate + Matter & Energy + Computers & Math + Fossils & Ruins + Science & Society * Articles + Health & Medicine + Mind & Brain + Plants & Animals + Space & Time + Earth & Climate + Matter & Energy + Computers & Math + Fossils & Ruins + Science & Society * Videos + Health & Medicine + Mind & Brain + Plants & Animals + Space & Time + Earth & Climate + Matter & Energy + Computers & Math + Fossils & Ruins + Science & Society * Images + Health & Medicine + Mind & Brain + Plants & Animals + Space & Time + Earth & Climate + Matter & Energy + Computers & Math + Fossils & Ruins + Science & Society * Books + Health & Medicine + Mind & Brain + Plants & Animals + Space & Time + Earth & Climate + Matter & Energy + Computers & Math + Fossils & Ruins + Science & Society Quantcast § #Pacific Standard » Feed Pacific Standard » Comments Feed [PS_Banner_Holiday2013.jpg] * subscribe today! * gift subscription * digital edition Pacific Standard November-December 2013 Cover Pacific Standard * SUBSCRIBE TO OUR NEWSLETTER Email address?______ * Comments ____________________ This field is for validation purposes and should be left unchanged. Submit * Politics * Health * Economy * Environment * Culture * Education * Science * Magazine + Current Issue + Subscribe + Digital Edition Placebo Effect Produces Higher Test Scores A new study finds it’s possible to trick people into doing better on a general-knowledge exam. [pills.jpg] (PHOTO: TRIFF/SHUTTERSTOCK) March 24, 2013 • By Tom Jacobs • 725 Flares Filament.io 725 Flares × Scientists are increasingly convinced of the power of the placebo effect. Believing that one is receiving treatment when you're not—say, in the form of a pill that supposedly contains a powerful drug that is actually just sugar—can produce surprisingly strong results, at least for some patients, some of the time. Newly published research suggests a placebo process can produce a similarly positive outcome for test-takers. In short, the belief that you have access to the answers makes it more likely you will get them right. “People have powerful psychological resources to deal with challenges, but those resources cannot always be used deliberately,” German psychologist Ulrich Weger and Australian psychologist Stephen Loughnan write in the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Just as a dummy pill can help people access their ability to tolerate pain, they report, a false conviction can help test-takers relax and improve their performance. Their experiment featured 40 undergraduates, each of whom took a 20-question general-knowledge text. Questions ranged from the numerical value of pi to the artist who created Guernica; participants were given four possible answers and instructed to pick the correct one. Before doing so, half the students were told that just before each question was asked, the correct answer was momentarily flashed onto the screen. They were informed that this happened too quickly for them to process the information consciously, but assured that it would register in their brains. A “demonstration” showed them the process in slow motion, reinforcing their belief that this was really happening. In fact, the researchers write, “the subliminally presented answers of this experimental phase were random letter strings.” No matter: “Participants in the placebo condition who believed they had been exposed to the correct answers subliminally scored higher than participants in the control condition,” Weger and Loughnan write. The researchers suspect the false assurance produced “a weakening of inhibitory mechanisms that normally impair performance on a task.” In other words, the participants’ belief that they had the answers allowed them to relax, focus—and answer more questions correctly. When you’re confident, “anxieties that have previously taxed cognitive resources … become available for other tasks and processes,” they write. Weger and Loughnan concede this is one small study, but it conforms with earlier research on confirmation bias and self-fulfilling prophecies. In this case, they note, the positive bias was not the product of internal self-talk, but rather an outside intervention—a mental sugar pill. As such, they speculate, this technique could come in handy in situations “where a participant’s genuine skills profile needs to be assessed, uncontaminated by the impact of self-incapacitating fears.” Did that job applicant’s low score reflect a lack of brainpower, or merely the anxiety of an inherently stressful situation? Some version of this experiment could be a fine way way to find out. For the rest of us, this study confirms that the best possible mantra to use before taking a test is: You know this. Related Posts * Ibuprofen Pills Fight Pain -- And You Don't Even Have to Take Them * (PHOTO COLLAGE: SHUTTERSTOCK) Mindfulness Training Boosts Test Scores Subscribe to Our Weekly Email Newsletter ____________________ Subscribe More like this: Findings Tags: Anxiety, Placebo Effect, Stress About Tom Jacobs Staff writer Tom Jacobs is a veteran journalist with more than 20 years experience at daily newspapers. He has served as a staff writer for The Los Angeles Daily News and the Santa Barbara News-Press. His work has also appeared in The Los Angeles Times, Chicago Tribune and Ventura County Star. Search this website Search Facebook Twitter RSS Pin It Share Google+ * Popular * Latest * Today Week Month All * Perils of a Model Minority: Where Did the Concept of the 'Asian F' Come From? Perils of a Model Minority: Where Did the Concept of the 'Asian F' Come From? * We Aren’t the World We Aren’t the World * From the Mouths of Rapists: The Lyrics of Robin Thicke’s 'Blurred Lines' From the Mouths of Rapists: The Lyrics of Robin Thicke’s 'Blurred Lines' * Why the Oscars Should Be Segregated by Gender Why the Oscars Should Be Segregated by Gender * That Armpit Odor Says a Lot About Who You Are That Armpit Odor Says a Lot About Who You Are * Israel Is Dying Israel Is Dying * What Does the Rapid Growth of Catholic Hospitals Mean for Reproductive Health? What Does the Rapid Growth of Catholic Hospitals Mean for Reproductive Health? * Why Don't We Use Condoms for Oral Sex? Why Don't We Use Condoms for Oral Sex? * Embrace Your Small Penis Embrace Your Small Penis * Why Don’t We Just Shoot Condemned Inmates? Why Don’t We Just Shoot Condemned Inmates? * Law Enforcement Deaths Are at a Record Low Law Enforcement Deaths Are at a Record Low * Why the Oscars Should Be Segregated by Gender Why the Oscars Should Be Segregated by Gender * Pet Selfies Are Not Selfies Pet Selfies Are Not Selfies * Perils of a Model Minority: Where Did the Concept of the 'Asian F' Come From? Perils of a Model Minority: Where Did the Concept of the 'Asian F' Come From? * You Know Who Else Uses Unpaid Interns? Colleges and Universities You Know Who Else Uses Unpaid Interns? Colleges and Universities * What to Watch for: Next Year's Biggest Stories in Politics What to Watch for: Next Year's Biggest Stories in Politics * Suburban Sprawl Spurs Innovation Suburban Sprawl Spurs Innovation * How Language Helps Us Understand Music Without Words How Language Helps Us Understand Music Without Words * Born With Unusual Bodies Born With Unusual Bodies * Urban Minor Leagues of Globalization Urban Minor Leagues of Globalization Ajax spinner Findings - Pacific Standard The Secret of Comedy Really Is Timing The Migraines and the Music: Was Wagner Inspired by Headache Pain? Long-Distance Relationships Really Can Work The Biggest Losers Aren’t Necessarily More Healthy Irony in Our Diets: Stigmatizing Obesity Increases Overeating About Us * About Us * Our Staff * Miller-McCune Staff * Our Founder * Contact Us * Write for Us * Advertise * Privacy Statement * Find a Magazine Follow Us * Pacific Standard @pacificstand * Maria Streshinsky @mstreshinsky * Nicholas Jackson @nbj914 * Ryan O'Hanlon @rwohan * Vince Beiser @vincelb * Tom Jacobs @tomjacobs_psmag * Michael Todd @mtodd_psmag * Marc Herman @marc_herman_ * Seth Masket @smotus Recent Stories This Year's Best Reporting on Education Does Netflix Really Bring Us Together? Hard Data to Back Up the Rhetoric on Recidivism San Francisco's Fortress Against Gentrification Return to top of page Copyright © 2014 · Pacific Standard § #Program in Placebo Studies & Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS) » Feed Program in Placebo Studies & Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS) » Comments Feed Program in Placebo Studies & Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS) » Welcome to PiPS Comments Feed About Us Recent News Program in Placebo Studies & Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS) Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center / Harvard Medical School Search ____________________ Search Main menu Skip to primary content Skip to secondary content * Home * About Us + Our Priorities & Mission + Team + Frequently Asked Questions * Research + Patient-Oriented Clinical Research + Research in Neuroscience & Molecular Biology + Research in the Social Sciences + Research Methodology + Initiatives in the Humanities and Bioethics * Training * Recent News * Media * Publications * Seminars + Robert Wood Johnson Foundation 2013-14 Series + Fall/Winter 2012-13 + September 2012 + Spring 2012 + Fall/Winter 2011 * Contact Us Welcome to PiPS For many years, the placebo effect was considered to be no more than a nuisance variable that needed to be controlled in clinical trials. Only recently have researchers redefined it as the key to understanding the healing that arises from medical ritual, the context of treatment, the patient-provider relationship and the power of imagination, trust and hope. Although our biomedical health care system often considers these humanistic dimensions of care as secondary to the administration of pharmaceuticals and procedures, the emerging field of placebo studies is producing scientific evidence that these more intangible elements of medicine may fundamentally contribute to the improvement of patient outcomes. The Program in Placebo Studies and the Therapeutic Encounter hosted at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center is the first research center to pursue placebo studies through interdisciplinary, translational research initiatives that bridge the basic, clinical and social sciences, as well as the humanities. Research PiPS researchers come from across Harvard University’s teaching hospitals and its Faculty of Arts and Sciences. This interdisciplinary identity allows researchers to draw upon multiple conceptual models and methodologies as they design studies that elucidate critical aspects of placebo studies. Read more… Training Karin at work PiPS seeks to establish itself as a center where young researchers can develop careers in placebo studies. Collaborations between senior and junior faculty are encouraged and many PiPS researchers are supported by NIH career awards and fellowships. Read more… Follow @Harvard_Placebo * Internal Proudly powered by WordPress § #prev next The Psychiatrist Skip to main page content * HOME * CURRENT * ARCHIVE * FEEDBACK * SUBSCRIBE * HELP Search for Keyword: ____________________ GO GO Advanced Search User Name ____________________ Password ____________________ Sign In Sign In * * Special article The placebo effect 1. Sundararajan Rajagopal, Consultant Psychiatrist 1. South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, Adamson Centre for Mental Health, St Thomas’ Hospital, London SE17EH, e-mail: Sundararajan.Rajagopal{at}slam.nhs.uk * Declaration of interest * None. Next Section The placebo effect is a fascinating phenomenon in clinical practice. Studies have shown that there is a significant placebo effect in a wide range of medical conditions including psychiatric disorders. This article looks at the background of the placebo effect, defines the common terms used, describes the various hypotheses that have been put forward to explain this seemingly inexplicable phenomenon and also covers the issue of using placebos in research trials, highlighting the important ethical dilemmas involved. Throughout, specific emphasis is given to psychiatry. Previous SectionNext Section Background The term placebo is derived from the Latin verb ‘placare’, ‘ to please’. The American anaesthetist Henry K. Beecher (1955) coined the term ‘ placebo effect’. He reported that, on average, about a third of patients with a range of conditions improved when they were given placebos. This subsequently led to the development of placebo-controlled trials, whereby a new drug is said to have significant benefit only if it shows superiority over placebo. The placebo effect has also been a source of recent interesting debate in psychiatry with some claiming that a considerable proportion of benefit from antidepressant medication derives from the placebo effect (Kirsch & Sapirstein, 1998), whereas others (Leutcher et al, 2002) have stressed that response to placebo and to antidepressants involves distinct biological mechanisms. Previous SectionNext Section Definitions In general, a placebo is an inert substance that has no inherent pharmacological activity. It looks, smells and tastes like the active drug with which it is compared. An ‘active placebo’ is one that has its own inherent effects but none for the condition that it is being given for (e.g. use of atropine as the control drug in trials of tricyclic antidepressants). A placebo need not always be pharmacological. It could be procedural, for example, sham electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), where the patient is anaesthetised but not given ECT. Surgical placebo is a procedure where the patient is anaesthetised and superficial procedures (e.g. skin incision, burr hole) are performed without the actual surgery. Placebo equivalents are also employed in complementary medicine. For example, sham acupuncture consists of needles placed at non-acupuncture points. A recent study (Linde et al, 2005) showed that real acupuncture was no more effective than sham acupuncture in reducing migraine headaches, although both interventions produced benefits compared with a waiting list control. Previous SectionNext Section Why does the placebo effect occur? Natural remission theory This states that the improvement that occurs with the administration of placebo is coincidental and would have occurred even without it. This theory explains the beneficial effects of placebo in short-lived conditions like common cold, headache, etc, but does not satisfactorily explain why even patients with chronic conditions such as hypertension or schizophrenia show improvement with placebo. An allied hypothesis is the ‘regression to the mean’ theory. Regression to the mean is a statistical concept; according to this, if an initial test result is extreme and if the test is repeated, statistically there is a greater likelihood for the second result to be closer to the mean than for it to be more extreme than the first result. Usually only patients who are significantly unwell (e.g. depression score above a certain cut-off point) are eligible to enter a trial. Hence, at follow-up they are more likely to show an improvement (depression score being closer to the mean than the first score) than a deterioration, owing to regression to the mean (McDonald et al, 1983). Classical (Pavlovian) conditioning In the original experiment of Pavlov, the dog salivated at the sound of the bell even without any food, as it had previously been conditioned to expect food by pairing the bell with food. Food is the unconditioned stimulus, salivation owing to food is the unconditioned response; the bell is the conditioned stimulus and salivation owing to the bell is the conditioned response. In a similar manner, patients who have had past experience of getting better with active medication may be conditioned to anticipate improvement by any subsequent prescription, including placebo. Using the classical conditioning analogy, the active medication is the unconditioned stimulus, improvement owing to active medication is the unconditioned response, the placebo is the conditioned stimulus, and improvement owing to placebo is the conditioned response. Other psychological factors Patient expectations are important in determining the placebo effect. Treatments that are perceived as being more powerful tend to have a stronger placebo effect than those that are perceived to be less so. Thus, placebo injections have more effect than oral placebos, capsules are perceived as being stronger than tablets, bright-coloured placebos are more effective than light-coloured ones larger placebos have more effect than smaller ones, and two placebos have more effect than one. Also, the status of the treating professional is directly related to the placebo effect. The same compound has been found to be more powerful if it is branded than when it is unbranded (Branthwaite & Cooper, 1981). In a novel study, Benedetti et al (2003) examined the impact of the patient’s awareness that they are having a certain treatment administered/withdrawn on the outcome. They studied three treatments in three groups of patients - intravenous morphine for post-thoracotomy pain, intravenous diazepam for post-thoracotomy anxiety and stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus for idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. In each group, some patients were informed of the fact that they were receiving the treatment (e.g. by a doctor administering the injection) but others were not aware as they received an infusion from an automatic pre-programmed machine. In all the groups, the efficacy of the respective interventions was greater when the patient was aware of the procedure than when they were not. Similarly, being aware that a treatment was being withdrawn worsened the symptoms much more than when the treatment was withdrawn without the patient’s knowledge. From a psychiatric point of view, neither the hidden administration nor hidden withdrawal of diazepam had any significant positive or negative effect respectively but the open administration of diazepam improved anxiety symptoms and open withdrawal worsened them. Role of endogenous opioids In a systematic review, ter Riet et al (1998) concluded that endogenous opioids (e.g. endorphins) play a significant role in mediating placebo-induced analgesia. Previous studies had shown that placebo-induced analgesia is partially reversed by administering the opioid antagonist naloxone (Grevert et al, 1983). There is also growing interest in the role of neurotransmitters, particularly dopamine, in placebo effects on mood and behaviour. Previous SectionNext Section Pattern of placebo improvement Among psychiatric disorders, the placebo effect has been most extensively studied in depression. ‘Pattern analyses’ have shown that the improvement as a result of placebo in depression tends to be abrupt, occurs early in treatment and is less likely to persist (Quitkin et al, 1991), whereas improvement in response to antidepressants tends to be gradual, occurs later and is more likely to persist. Even among patients apparently responding to the active drug, if the pattern of improvement is consistent with a placebo response (i.e. abrupt and early), the improvement tends to be short-lived. Stewart et al (1998) investigated whether they could predict relapse of depression from the initial pattern of response. Patients who had responded to treatment with fluoxetine for 12–14 weeks were then randomly allocated to continuation/maintenance treatment for 50 weeks with either placebo or fluoxetine. Those patients who had shown a placebo pattern of improvement during the initial fluoxetine phase relapsed in a similar manner whether they continued on fluoxetine or were switched to placebo, but patients who had shown a true drug pattern of improvement relapsed more if they were switched to placebo in the maintenance phase. This study adds strength to the hypothesis that, even among drug responders, only a certain proportion will benefit from maintenance treatment. Hrobjartsson & Gotzsche (2001) conducted a major systematic review of placebo-controlled trials involving 40 clinical conditions, including hypertension, asthma, pain, depression, schizophrenia, anxiety and epilepsy. They concluded that placebos tended to have no significant effects on binary outcomes, and possibly had small beneficial effects on continuous subjective outcomes and in the treatment of pain. Previous SectionNext Section Use of placebos in clinical trials It is generally accepted that a double-blind randomised controlled trial (RCT) is the best research method to study the efficacy of clinical interventions. However, the use of placebos for conditions for which effective treatments are already available raises an important ethical question. Should a new treatment be compared with an established treatment or should it only have to demonstrate superiority over placebo in order to be accepted as another effective treatment? Rothman & Michels (1994) have criticised the use of placebo-controlled trials to test new drugs for conditions with potentially irreversible consequences, such as onchocerciasis and rheumatoid arthritis, when established treatments for these conditions already exist. Death by suicide is associated with major psychiatric disorders such as depression, and the use of placebo-controlled trials to test the efficacy of new drugs is fraught with ethical issues. Another important question involves the masking of the double-blind trials. Margraf et al (1991) reported that a majority of patients in a double-blind study of alprazolam v. imipramine v. placebo could correctly guess whether they were on an active drug or placebo. In addition, the ‘masked’ assessors were even able to distinguish between the two active drugs. Informed consent entails the patients being made aware that they will be receiving either an active drug or placebo. Therefore, it may not require more than just monitoring one’s side-effects closely to accurately determine whether one is on active medication or placebo. In addition to ethical issues, RCTs with a placebo control group have other limitations. RCTs only demonstrate statistical significance. If the sample size is very large, even if the difference in clinical outcome between the two groups is small and clinically insignificant, it may be detected as being significant by the statistical test. In any RCT, the placebo is made by the manufacturer of the active drug. Hence, placebos used in one study will be different in form (size, shape, tablet/capsule, etc.) from those used in another study, depending on the form of the active drug. This may account for the wide variation in placebo response observed for the same condition. Previous SectionNext Section Side-effects of placebos When a placebo produces prominent side-effects it is known as a ‘ nocebo’. The term ‘nocebo effect’ encompasses the negative consequences resulting from the administration of a placebo. In placebo-controlled studies of psychotropic drugs, the placebos tend to cause a similar range of side-effects as the active drugs but usually with a much lower incidence rate. Non-specific side-effects, such as headache and nausea, tend to be more common than more specific ones such as acute dystonia or QT prolongation. ‘Placebo sag’ refers to the attenuation of the placebo effect with repeated use (Peck & Coleman, 1991). There are historical reports of placebo dependence (Vinar, 1969). The nocebo effect clearly illustrates the role of patient expectations in perceived side-effects. Usually patients included in trials of psychotropic medication have already received previous treatment with active medication in the past, as most major psychiatric disorders tend to follow a chronic course. Hence, even if they are given placebo this time, they may anticipate side-effects similar to those that they experienced when they were receiving treatment with the active drug. Also, patients may be influenced by the list of side-effects experienced by their friends or relatives who have received such treatment in the past, and by the list of potential side-effects described by the researchers before obtaining informed consent. Just as doubts have been cast on the beneficial effects of the placebo, so have questions been raised about the nocebo effect. Even healthy people who are not taking any medication have been shown to have a high prevalence of a range of symptoms which are similar to the side-effects investigated during RCTs. Thus, side-effects reported by patients on placebo may be a reflection of pre-existing or spontaneously occurring symptoms rather than being placebo-induced. Similarly, RCTs may be overestimating the side-effects (especially the nonspecific ones) of active drugs. However, the nocebo effect is not purely psychological. It has been shown that nocebo hyperalgesia (i.e. an increase in pain as a result of placebo) is mediated by cholecystokinin and is abolished by the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide (Benedetti et al, 1997). In a systematic review of double-blind RCTs comparing fluoxetine and placebo, Casper et al (2001) found similar rates of placebo response in men and women but slightly more nocebo effects in women. Previous SectionNext Section Conclusions Despite half a century having passed since its inclusion in modern medicine, the placebo effect is still poorly understood. Beecher’s (1955) original study, which showed an overall average placebo response of 35%, has been strongly criticised for major methodological shortcomings (Kienle & Kiene, 1997). All discussion regarding placebos is based on the assumption that they are inert. But are they really so? Placebos are generally referred to as ‘ sugar pills’; sugar is not chemically inert. Similarly, the tablet coating or the capsule covering are not inert. Hence, the possibility that the ‘inert’ chemical in the placebo may be relevant to the condition being studied should not be dismissed. Whatever the reasons for the placebo effect, the most important message for clinicians is that just because someone responds to a placebo does not mean that the initial ailment for which they sought help was false. * © 2006 Royal College of Psychiatrists Previous Section References 1. ↵ BEECHER, H. K. (1955) The powerful placebo. JAMA, 159, 1602 -1606. CrossRefMedline 2. ↵ BENEDETTI, F., AMANZIO, M., CASADIO, C., et al (1997) Blockade of nocebo hyperalgesia by the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide. Pain, 71, 135 -140. CrossRefMedline 3. ↵ BENEDETTI, F., MAGGI, G., LOPIANO, L., et al (2003) Open versus hidden medical treatments: the patients’ knowledge about a therapy affects the therapy outcome. Prevention & Treatment, 6. http://content.apa.org/journals/pre/6/1/1 4. ↵ BRANTHWAITE, A. & COOPER, P. (1981) Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches. BMJ, 282, 1576 -1578. Abstract/FREE Full Text 5. ↵ CASPER, R. C., TOLLEFSON, G. D. & NILSSON, M. E. (2001) No gender differences in placebo responses of patients with major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 15, 158 -160. 6. ↵ GREVERT, P., ALBERT, L. H. & GOLDSTEIN, A. (1983) Partial antagonism of placebo analgesia by naloxone. Pain, 16, 129 -143. CrossRefMedline 7. ↵ HROBJARTSSON, A. & GOTZSCHE, P. C. (2001) Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 1594 -1602. CrossRefMedline 8. ↵ KIENLE, G. S. & KIENE, H. (1997) The powerful placebo effect: fact or fiction? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 50, 1311 -1318. CrossRefMedline 9. ↵ KIRSCH, I. & SAPIRSTEIN, G. (1998) Listening to Prozac but hearing placebo: a meta-analysis of antidepressant medication. Prevention & Treatment, 1, http://content.apa.org/journals/pre/1/1/2 10. LEUCHTER, A.F., COOK, I.A., WITTE, E. A., et al (2002) Changes in brain function of depressed subjects during treatment with placebo. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 122 -129. CrossRefMedline 11. ↵ LINDE, K., STRENG, A., JURGENS, S., et al (2005) Acupuncture for patients with migraine. JAMA, 293, 2118 -2125. CrossRefMedline 12. ↵ MARGRAF, J., EHLERS, A., ROTH, W.T., et al (1991) How ‘blind’are double-blind studies? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 184 -187. CrossRefMedline 13. ↵ McDONALD, C. J., MAZZUCA, S. A. & McCABE, G. P., Jr (1983) How much of the placebo‘effect’ is really statistical regression? Statistical Medicine, 2, 417-427. 14. ↵ PECK, C. & COLEMAN, G. (1991) Implications of placebo theory for clinical research and practice in pain management.Theoretical Medicine, 12, 247 -270. CrossRefMedline 15. ↵ QUITKIN, F. M., RABKIN, J. G., STEWART, J.W., et al (1991) Heterogeneity of clinical response during placebo treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 193 -196. Medline 16. ↵ ROTHMAN, K. J. & MICHELS, K. B. (1994) The continuing unethical use of placebo controls. New England Journal of Medicine, 331, 394 -398. CrossRefMedline 17. ↵ STEWART, J.W., QUITKIN, F. M., McGRATH, P. J. (1998) Use of pattern analysis to predict differential relapse of remitted patients with major depression during 1 year of treatment with fluoxetine or placebo. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 334 -343. CrossRefMedline 18. ↵ Ter RIET, G., De CRAEN, A. J., De BOER, A., et al (1998) Is placebo analgesia mediated by endogenous opioids? A systematic review. Pain, 76, 273 -275. CrossRefMedline 19. ↵ VINAR, O. (1969) Dependence on a placebo: a case report. British Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 1189 -1190. Abstract/FREE Full Text « Previous | Next article Table of Contents This article 1. The Psychiatrist (2006) 30: 185-188 doi: 10.1192/pb.30.5.185 1. » Full text 2. PDF Services 1. Email this article to a colleague 2. Alert me when this article is cited 3. Alert me if a correction is posted 4. Alert me when eletters are published 5. Similar articles in this journal 6. Similar articles in Web of Science 7. Download to citation manager 8. Get copyright permission Responses 1. Submit a response 2. No responses published Citing Articles 1. Load citing article information 2. Citing articles via Web of Science 3. Citing articles via Google Scholar Google Scholar 1. Articles by Rajagopal, S. 2. Search for related content PubMed 1. Articles by Rajagopal, S. Related Content 1. Load related web page information Navigate This Article 1. Top 2. Background 3. Definitions 4. Why does the placebo effect occur? 5. Pattern of placebo improvement 6. Use of placebos in clinical trials 7. Side-effects of placebos 8. Conclusions 9. References This Month's Issue December 2013, Volume 37 Issue 12 1. Current Issue 1. Alert me to new issues of The Psychiatrist * ABOUT THE JOURNAL * SUBSCRIBE * MANUSCRIPT SUBMISSION * RESOURCES FOR AUTHORS * PERMISSIONS AND REPRINTS * OTHER RCPSYCH PUBLICATIONS * ALERTS AND RSS FEEDS Royal College of Psychiatrists Most * Viewed 1. Medical records: Disclosing confidential clinical information 2. Assessment of mental capacity: a flow chart guide 3. Lithium and pregnancy 4. The placebo effect 5. The treatment of shell-shock: Cognitive therapy before its time » View all Most Viewed articles * Cited 1. Consultant psychiatrists who retired prematurely in 1995 and 1996 2. Sources of stress, psychological distress and burnout in psychiatrists: Comparison of junior doctors, senior registrars and consultants 3. The results of a multi-centre audit of the prescribing of antipsychotic drugs for in-patients in the UK 4. A survey of stress in psychiatrists working in the Wessex Region 5. Religion and mental health: what should psychiatrists do? » View all Most Cited articles © 2014 The Royal College of Psychiatrists * Online ISSN: 1758-3217 * Print ISSN: 1758-3209 § #RSS 2.0 RSS .92 Atom 0.3 Bad Science » placebo Category Feed Bad Science Search * ____________________ Search TED Talk This New Book This Great Book T-shirts Categories * ABPI (3) * academic pr (4) * academic publishing (6) * adverts (45) * africa (28) * aids (6) * alltrials campaign (11) * alternative medicine (190) + acupuncture (5) + herbal remedies (20) + homeopathy (52) + nutritionists (88) * anecdotes (2) * annual roundup (1) * aric sigman (2) * authority (1) * bad science (656) * badpharma (3) * badscience (14) * BANT (2) * big pharma (36) * blue monday (3) * book (7) * book reviews (3) * brain gym (11) * brainiac (6) * bullying (3) * cash-for-"stories" (14) * celebs (13) * childishness (1) * chocolate (6) * churnalism (8) * climate change (6) * competing interests (6) * conflict of interest (3) * cosmetics (13) * craig sams (2) * crime (2) * dangers (27) * danie krugel (1) * data (1) * death (6) * denialism (1) * detox (7) * dna (8) * dodgy academic press releases (3) * doing nothing (1) * dore (4) * drurrrgs (12) * duplicate publication (1) * economics (3) * electrosensitivity (20) * epidemiology (2) * equazen (13) * ESHLSG (1) * evidence (19) * evidence based policy (13) * evolutionary psychology (4) * fish oil (20) * fraud (1) * fuel pr (1) * geek (1) * genetic nonsense (1) * ghostwriters (3) * gillian mckeith (25) * Global Radio (2) * gm (2) * government reports (4) * great popularisers of science (2) * guest writers (1) * hate mail (9) * heroes (6) * heroes of bad science (6) * Hi-Fi (2) * hiding data (4) * history of quackery (2) * ID (1) * ions (5) * irrationality research (14) * jeni barnett (3) * jonathan leake (1) * journal club (14) * just a blog (2) * laws (2) * LBC (3) * legal chill (15) * letters (18) * libel (14) * magnets (20) * manufacturing doubt (1) * matthias rath (9) * media (162) + bbc (35) + channel 4 (11) + channel five (4) + evening standard (4) + express (20) + guardian (3) + independent (21) + ITV (6) + mail (45) + mirror (15) + news of the world (1) + sun (6) + telegraph (30) + times (36) * media research (2) * medicalisation (14) * methods (3) * MiniBlog (1) * miracles (2) * MMR (44) * models (1) * mondo academico (3) * MRSA (6) * nanniebots (3) * neuro nonsense (2) * neurostuff (3) * new scientist (4) * not bad science (9) * numerical context (11) * onanism (58) * open methods (1) * organic (1) * oxygen (9) * patents (2) * patrick holford (14) * penises (6) * perpetual motion (4) * PhDs, doctors, and qualifications (34) * phone stalking (4) * placebo (21) * podcast (18) * politics (30) * postmodernist bollocks (6) * powerwatch – alasdair philips (9) * pr guff (4) * predictions (1) * presenting numbers (3) * procrastination (1) * pseudodiagnoses (1) * psychic nonsense (1) * psychology of woo (2) * publication bias (18) * qlink (1) * quantum physics (12) * rape (1) * references (59) * regulating media (2) * regulating nonsense (6) * regulating research (33) * religion (26) * retractions (1) * revenge (1) * risk (1) * roger coghill (2) * roland kapferer (2) * roundup (1) * scare stories (52) * schools (7) * science comms (2) * scienciness (1) * screening (2) * secret data (3) * show your working (1) * small blogs (1) * smears (1) * space (4) * spin (2) * statistics (82) * Stephen Whitehead (1) * stifling criticism (10) * structured data (5) * subgroup analysis (3) * suicide (2) * surveillance (1) * survey data (5) * susan greenfield (1) * systematic reviews (9) * tamiflu (1) * teaching resources (1) * tobacco (1) * toys (1) * trial registers (6) * uncertainty (4) * utter nonsense (3) * vaccines (2) * very basic science (52) * water (18) * weight loss (10) * whistleblowers (1) The golden arse beam method. July 9th, 2011 by Ben Goldacre in alternative medicine, irrationality research, placebo | 15 Comments » Ben Goldacre, The Guardian, Saturday 9 July 2011 Since I was a teenager, whenever I have a pivotal life event coming – an exam, or an interview – I perform a ritual. I sit cross-legged on the floor, and I imagine an enormous golden beam of energy coming out of my arse. Read the rest of this entry » Placebo! Nocebo! April 17th, 2010 by Ben Goldacre in onanism, placebo, podcast | 29 Comments » Hi, two quick videos… one on the mighty placebo effect, via NHS Choices, one on the nocebo effect from Nerdstock. Read the rest of this entry » All bow before the mighty power of the nocebo effect November 28th, 2009 by Ben Goldacre in homeopathy, placebo | 70 Comments » Ben Goldacre, Saturday 28 November 2009, The Guardian This week the parliamentary science and technology select committee looked into the evidence behind the MHRA’s decision to allow homeopathy sugar pill labels to make medical claims without evidence of efficacy, and the funding of homeopathy on the NHS. There were some comedy highlights, as you might expect from any serious enquiry into an industry where sugar pills have healing powers conferred upon them by being shaken with one drop of the ingredient which has been diluted, so extremely, that it equates to one molecule of the substance in a sphere of water whose diameter is roughly the distance from the earth to the sun. Read the rest of this entry » Part two of my Radio 4 show on the placebo effect, 9pm tonight (Monday) August 25th, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in bad science, onanism, placebo, podcast | 31 Comments » image So tonight at 9pm on BBC Radio 4 (Monday) you can hear the second episode of my two-part miniseries on the placebo effect, one of the most effective and neglected evidence based treatments known to man. In this show we look at the ethical and practical implications of research into the placebo effect, and discuss whether it’s okay – or even necessary – to lie to patients. The answer, from me at any rate, is “no”. Read the rest of this entry » Think yourself thin… August 23rd, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in bad science, placebo | 33 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian, Saturday August 23 2008 What I particularly enjoy is the spectacle of fat people – ideally drinking beer – watching television, while somewhere on the other side of the world citizens of all nations are getting some nice exercise in the Olympics (throwing javelins, jumping over metal bars, climbing lamp posts with banners, and running away from the water cannon). These are the people I imagine paying for gyms they never visit, while I am cheerfully cycling to work and carrying the shopping up the stairs. Read the rest of this entry » My Placebo programme on BBC Radio 4 August 18th, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in bad science, onanism, placebo | 30 Comments » [060721_pills_vmedwidec.jpg] I’ve been so busy I completely failed to spot that this show went out earlier this evening. It’s a smashing programme I made with Matt Silver from the BBC Radio 4 Science Unit on the placebo effect. We charge through some of the most fun experiments in the field, and in part two we get all philosophical about what it means for mankind. Read the rest of this entry » All bow before the might of the placebo effect, it is the coolest strangest thing in medicine March 1st, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in homeopathy, placebo, regulating research | 31 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian, Saturday March 1 2008 It was fun to hear universal jubilation over the new meta-analysis showing once again that some antidepressants aren’t much cop in mild or moderate depression: most of all on the Today programme, where a newsreader said the industry was contesting the study on the basis that it was not in line “with patient experience”. I’ve always said that homeopaths mimic big pharma in their marketing spiel, but this is the first time I’ve seen it done the other way around, so bravo to pill peddlers of all shades. Read the rest of this entry » Medicalisation – don’t take it lying down. September 29th, 2007 by Ben Goldacre in acupuncture, bad science, medicalisation, placebo | 69 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian Saturday September 29 2007 One thing that always fascinates me, as I tug on my pipe in this armchair, is how reductionist, how mechanical, how sciencey and medical we like our stories about the body to be. This week a major new study was published on acupuncture. Many newspapers said it showed acupuncture performing better than medical treatment: in fact it was 8 million times more interesting than that. Read the rest of this entry » Acupuncture and back pain: some interesting background references September 25th, 2007 by Ben Goldacre in acupuncture, bad science, placebo | 96 Comments » I was just on Radio 4′s PM program talking about the acupuncture study that’s in the news today, you can listen to it here (37 minutes in to the programme): www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/news/pm/ Here are some references and background bits and bobs. The paper itself was very interesting. It took 1200 people, with an average of 8 years back pain each: we can assume not been helped by biomedical treatments. They were split into three groups: one group had medical treatment; one group had proper, real, bells and whistles, needles in the “meridiens” acupuncture; and one group were treated with pretend acupuncture. Read the rest of this entry » Homeopathy gives you Aids September 15th, 2007 by Ben Goldacre in homeopathy, MMR, placebo | 48 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian September 15th, 2007 Okay now look: there’s nothing wrong with the idea of homeopaths giving out sugar pills. The placebo effect can be very powerful, because it’s not just about the pill, it’s about the cultural meaning of the treatment: so we know from research that four placebo sugar pills a day are more effective than two for eradicating gastric ulcers (and that’s not subjective, you measure ulcers by putting a camera into your stomach); we know that salt water injections are a more effective treatment for pain than sugar pills, not because salt water injections are medically active, but because injections are a more dramatic intervention; we know that green sugar pills are a more effective anxiety treatment than red ones, not because of any biomechanical effect of the dyes, but because of the cultural meanings of the colours green and red. We even know that packaging can be beneficial. Read the rest of this entry » « Older Entries Stuff * About Dr Ben Goldacre + - Upcoming public talks + - Speaking engagements * Books + - Bad Pharma + - Bad Science * What You Can Find Here + - Audio and Video + - Bad Science Shop I love them Powered by Positive Archives * December 2013 * October 2013 * July 2013 * June 2013 * May 2013 * April 2013 * March 2013 * February 2013 * January 2013 * December 2012 * October 2012 * September 2012 * June 2012 * April 2012 * March 2012 * December 2011 * November 2011 * October 2011 * September 2011 * August 2011 * July 2011 * June 2011 * May 2011 * April 2011 * March 2011 * February 2011 * January 2011 * December 2010 * November 2010 * October 2010 * September 2010 * August 2010 * July 2010 * June 2010 * May 2010 * April 2010 * March 2010 * February 2010 * January 2010 * December 2009 * November 2009 * October 2009 * September 2009 * August 2009 * July 2009 * June 2009 * May 2009 * April 2009 * March 2009 * February 2009 * January 2009 * December 2008 * November 2008 * October 2008 * September 2008 * August 2008 * July 2008 * June 2008 * May 2008 * April 2008 * March 2008 * February 2008 * January 2008 * December 2007 * November 2007 * October 2007 * September 2007 * August 2007 * July 2007 * June 2007 * May 2007 * April 2007 * March 2007 * February 2007 * January 2007 * December 2006 * November 2006 * October 2006 * September 2006 * August 2006 * July 2006 * June 2006 * May 2006 * April 2006 * March 2006 * February 2006 * January 2006 * December 2005 * November 2005 * October 2005 * September 2005 * August 2005 * July 2005 * June 2005 * May 2005 * April 2005 * March 2005 * February 2005 * January 2005 * December 2004 * November 2004 * October 2004 * September 2004 * August 2004 * July 2004 * June 2004 * May 2004 * April 2004 * March 2004 * February 2004 * January 2004 * December 2003 * November 2003 * October 2003 * September 2003 * August 2003 * July 2003 * June 2003 * May 2003 * April 2003 * February 2003 * July 2002 * January 2000 * January 1007 Meta * Register * Log in * RSS * Comments RSS * Valid XHTML * XFN § #RSS 2.0 RSS .92 Atom 0.3 Bad Science » placebo Category Feed Bad Science Search * ____________________ Search TED Talk This New Book This Great Book T-shirts Categories * ABPI (3) * academic pr (4) * academic publishing (6) * adverts (45) * africa (28) * aids (6) * alltrials campaign (11) * alternative medicine (190) + acupuncture (5) + herbal remedies (20) + homeopathy (52) + nutritionists (88) * anecdotes (2) * annual roundup (1) * aric sigman (2) * authority (1) * bad science (656) * badpharma (3) * badscience (14) * BANT (2) * big pharma (36) * blue monday (3) * book (7) * book reviews (3) * brain gym (11) * brainiac (6) * bullying (3) * cash-for-"stories" (14) * celebs (13) * childishness (1) * chocolate (6) * churnalism (8) * climate change (6) * competing interests (6) * conflict of interest (3) * cosmetics (13) * craig sams (2) * crime (2) * dangers (27) * danie krugel (1) * data (1) * death (6) * denialism (1) * detox (7) * dna (8) * dodgy academic press releases (3) * doing nothing (1) * dore (4) * drurrrgs (12) * duplicate publication (1) * economics (3) * electrosensitivity (20) * epidemiology (2) * equazen (13) * ESHLSG (1) * evidence (19) * evidence based policy (13) * evolutionary psychology (4) * fish oil (20) * fraud (1) * fuel pr (1) * geek (1) * genetic nonsense (1) * ghostwriters (3) * gillian mckeith (25) * Global Radio (2) * gm (2) * government reports (4) * great popularisers of science (2) * guest writers (1) * hate mail (9) * heroes (6) * heroes of bad science (6) * Hi-Fi (2) * hiding data (4) * history of quackery (2) * ID (1) * ions (5) * irrationality research (14) * jeni barnett (3) * jonathan leake (1) * journal club (14) * just a blog (2) * laws (2) * LBC (3) * legal chill (15) * letters (18) * libel (14) * magnets (20) * manufacturing doubt (1) * matthias rath (9) * media (162) + bbc (35) + channel 4 (11) + channel five (4) + evening standard (4) + express (20) + guardian (3) + independent (21) + ITV (6) + mail (45) + mirror (15) + news of the world (1) + sun (6) + telegraph (30) + times (36) * media research (2) * medicalisation (14) * methods (3) * MiniBlog (1) * miracles (2) * MMR (44) * models (1) * mondo academico (3) * MRSA (6) * nanniebots (3) * neuro nonsense (2) * neurostuff (3) * new scientist (4) * not bad science (9) * numerical context (11) * onanism (58) * open methods (1) * organic (1) * oxygen (9) * patents (2) * patrick holford (14) * penises (6) * perpetual motion (4) * PhDs, doctors, and qualifications (34) * phone stalking (4) * placebo (21) * podcast (18) * politics (30) * postmodernist bollocks (6) * powerwatch – alasdair philips (9) * pr guff (4) * predictions (1) * presenting numbers (3) * procrastination (1) * pseudodiagnoses (1) * psychic nonsense (1) * psychology of woo (2) * publication bias (18) * qlink (1) * quantum physics (12) * rape (1) * references (59) * regulating media (2) * regulating nonsense (6) * regulating research (33) * religion (26) * retractions (1) * revenge (1) * risk (1) * roger coghill (2) * roland kapferer (2) * roundup (1) * scare stories (52) * schools (7) * science comms (2) * scienciness (1) * screening (2) * secret data (3) * show your working (1) * small blogs (1) * smears (1) * space (4) * spin (2) * statistics (82) * Stephen Whitehead (1) * stifling criticism (10) * structured data (5) * subgroup analysis (3) * suicide (2) * surveillance (1) * survey data (5) * susan greenfield (1) * systematic reviews (9) * tamiflu (1) * teaching resources (1) * tobacco (1) * toys (1) * trial registers (6) * uncertainty (4) * utter nonsense (3) * vaccines (2) * very basic science (52) * water (18) * weight loss (10) * whistleblowers (1) The golden arse beam method. July 9th, 2011 by Ben Goldacre in alternative medicine, irrationality research, placebo | 15 Comments » Ben Goldacre, The Guardian, Saturday 9 July 2011 Since I was a teenager, whenever I have a pivotal life event coming – an exam, or an interview – I perform a ritual. I sit cross-legged on the floor, and I imagine an enormous golden beam of energy coming out of my arse. Read the rest of this entry » Placebo! Nocebo! April 17th, 2010 by Ben Goldacre in onanism, placebo, podcast | 29 Comments » Hi, two quick videos… one on the mighty placebo effect, via NHS Choices, one on the nocebo effect from Nerdstock. Read the rest of this entry » All bow before the mighty power of the nocebo effect November 28th, 2009 by Ben Goldacre in homeopathy, placebo | 70 Comments » Ben Goldacre, Saturday 28 November 2009, The Guardian This week the parliamentary science and technology select committee looked into the evidence behind the MHRA’s decision to allow homeopathy sugar pill labels to make medical claims without evidence of efficacy, and the funding of homeopathy on the NHS. There were some comedy highlights, as you might expect from any serious enquiry into an industry where sugar pills have healing powers conferred upon them by being shaken with one drop of the ingredient which has been diluted, so extremely, that it equates to one molecule of the substance in a sphere of water whose diameter is roughly the distance from the earth to the sun. Read the rest of this entry » Part two of my Radio 4 show on the placebo effect, 9pm tonight (Monday) August 25th, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in bad science, onanism, placebo, podcast | 31 Comments » image So tonight at 9pm on BBC Radio 4 (Monday) you can hear the second episode of my two-part miniseries on the placebo effect, one of the most effective and neglected evidence based treatments known to man. In this show we look at the ethical and practical implications of research into the placebo effect, and discuss whether it’s okay – or even necessary – to lie to patients. The answer, from me at any rate, is “no”. Read the rest of this entry » Think yourself thin… August 23rd, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in bad science, placebo | 33 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian, Saturday August 23 2008 What I particularly enjoy is the spectacle of fat people – ideally drinking beer – watching television, while somewhere on the other side of the world citizens of all nations are getting some nice exercise in the Olympics (throwing javelins, jumping over metal bars, climbing lamp posts with banners, and running away from the water cannon). These are the people I imagine paying for gyms they never visit, while I am cheerfully cycling to work and carrying the shopping up the stairs. Read the rest of this entry » My Placebo programme on BBC Radio 4 August 18th, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in bad science, onanism, placebo | 30 Comments » [060721_pills_vmedwidec.jpg] I’ve been so busy I completely failed to spot that this show went out earlier this evening. It’s a smashing programme I made with Matt Silver from the BBC Radio 4 Science Unit on the placebo effect. We charge through some of the most fun experiments in the field, and in part two we get all philosophical about what it means for mankind. Read the rest of this entry » All bow before the might of the placebo effect, it is the coolest strangest thing in medicine March 1st, 2008 by Ben Goldacre in homeopathy, placebo, regulating research | 31 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian, Saturday March 1 2008 It was fun to hear universal jubilation over the new meta-analysis showing once again that some antidepressants aren’t much cop in mild or moderate depression: most of all on the Today programme, where a newsreader said the industry was contesting the study on the basis that it was not in line “with patient experience”. I’ve always said that homeopaths mimic big pharma in their marketing spiel, but this is the first time I’ve seen it done the other way around, so bravo to pill peddlers of all shades. Read the rest of this entry » Medicalisation – don’t take it lying down. September 29th, 2007 by Ben Goldacre in acupuncture, bad science, medicalisation, placebo | 69 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian Saturday September 29 2007 One thing that always fascinates me, as I tug on my pipe in this armchair, is how reductionist, how mechanical, how sciencey and medical we like our stories about the body to be. This week a major new study was published on acupuncture. Many newspapers said it showed acupuncture performing better than medical treatment: in fact it was 8 million times more interesting than that. Read the rest of this entry » Acupuncture and back pain: some interesting background references September 25th, 2007 by Ben Goldacre in acupuncture, bad science, placebo | 96 Comments » I was just on Radio 4′s PM program talking about the acupuncture study that’s in the news today, you can listen to it here (37 minutes in to the programme): www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/news/pm/ Here are some references and background bits and bobs. The paper itself was very interesting. It took 1200 people, with an average of 8 years back pain each: we can assume not been helped by biomedical treatments. They were split into three groups: one group had medical treatment; one group had proper, real, bells and whistles, needles in the “meridiens” acupuncture; and one group were treated with pretend acupuncture. Read the rest of this entry » Homeopathy gives you Aids September 15th, 2007 by Ben Goldacre in homeopathy, MMR, placebo | 48 Comments » Ben Goldacre The Guardian September 15th, 2007 Okay now look: there’s nothing wrong with the idea of homeopaths giving out sugar pills. The placebo effect can be very powerful, because it’s not just about the pill, it’s about the cultural meaning of the treatment: so we know from research that four placebo sugar pills a day are more effective than two for eradicating gastric ulcers (and that’s not subjective, you measure ulcers by putting a camera into your stomach); we know that salt water injections are a more effective treatment for pain than sugar pills, not because salt water injections are medically active, but because injections are a more dramatic intervention; we know that green sugar pills are a more effective anxiety treatment than red ones, not because of any biomechanical effect of the dyes, but because of the cultural meanings of the colours green and red. We even know that packaging can be beneficial. Read the rest of this entry » « Older Entries Stuff * About Dr Ben Goldacre + - Upcoming public talks + - Speaking engagements * Books + - Bad Pharma + - Bad Science * What You Can Find Here + - Audio and Video + - Bad Science Shop I love them Powered by Positive Archives * December 2013 * October 2013 * July 2013 * June 2013 * May 2013 * April 2013 * March 2013 * February 2013 * January 2013 * December 2012 * October 2012 * September 2012 * June 2012 * April 2012 * March 2012 * December 2011 * November 2011 * October 2011 * September 2011 * August 2011 * July 2011 * June 2011 * May 2011 * April 2011 * March 2011 * February 2011 * January 2011 * December 2010 * November 2010 * October 2010 * September 2010 * August 2010 * July 2010 * June 2010 * May 2010 * April 2010 * March 2010 * February 2010 * January 2010 * December 2009 * November 2009 * October 2009 * September 2009 * August 2009 * July 2009 * June 2009 * May 2009 * April 2009 * March 2009 * February 2009 * January 2009 * December 2008 * November 2008 * October 2008 * September 2008 * August 2008 * July 2008 * June 2008 * May 2008 * April 2008 * March 2008 * February 2008 * January 2008 * December 2007 * November 2007 * October 2007 * September 2007 * August 2007 * July 2007 * June 2007 * May 2007 * April 2007 * March 2007 * February 2007 * January 2007 * December 2006 * November 2006 * October 2006 * September 2006 * August 2006 * July 2006 * June 2006 * May 2006 * April 2006 * March 2006 * February 2006 * January 2006 * December 2005 * November 2005 * October 2005 * September 2005 * August 2005 * July 2005 * June 2005 * May 2005 * April 2005 * March 2005 * February 2005 * January 2005 * December 2004 * November 2004 * October 2004 * September 2004 * August 2004 * July 2004 * June 2004 * May 2004 * April 2004 * March 2004 * February 2004 * January 2004 * December 2003 * November 2003 * October 2003 * September 2003 * August 2003 * July 2003 * June 2003 * May 2003 * April 2003 * February 2003 * July 2002 * January 2000 * January 1007 Meta * Register * Log in * RSS * Comments RSS * Valid XHTML * XFN § Biology 202 1999 First Web Reports On Serendip This paper reflects the research and thoughts of a student at the time the paper was written for a course at Bryn Mawr College. Like other materials on Serendip, it is not intended to be "authoritative" but rather to help others further develop their own explorations. Web links were active as of the time the paper was posted but are not updated. Contribute Thoughts | Search Serendip for Other Papers | Serendip Home Page Therapeutic Placebo Effect: A Mind/Body Connection Patricia Anne Kinser __________________________________________________________________ reformatted from original at http://students.haverford.edu/pkinser/neuro-placebo.htm __________________________________________________________________ Imagine you go to your doctor for chronic back pain and she tells you that she's going to give you a drug, yet she's not sure of its effectiveness because only approximately 40% of her patients have found it to be beneficial. How sure will you be that the outcome of this treatment will be positive? However, what if your doctor tells you she is giving you the newest, most beneficial drug treatment on the market and that she is very sure of how helpful it will be? Imagine the difference just a simple positive statement from your doctor will make when you take the pill every day. Not only will you be trusting of your treatment, but that trust will lead you to be confident ^(Endnote 1) that you will feel better - and in most cases, you will. This is an example of the placebo effect - a self-made natural healing response of the body. No matter the fact that in both instances your doctor is giving you the same medication, her belief and yours in the treatment will most likely result in more positive effects. Many people argue that alternative methods of healing (such as hypnosis, therapeutic touch, homeopathic remedies, etc) are basically a placebo effect taking place. Yet, doesn't this fact prove the power of our minds both in health and in issues of pain management? Most modern scientists tend to separate the mind from the body, at least implicitly, suggesting the "mind" is simply a construct with little meaning. The one-cause, one- cure philosophy of science today often disregards the role of the mind in health and healing. Yet many of us even daily acknowledge the power of the mind when we say "oh, you're not sick- it's all in your mind". The placebo effect is not totally understood, yet this fact should not lead anyone to believe that its effects should be discounted. The placebo effect has been documented to be very powerful. A placebo is a medicine or other kind of treatment that seems therapeutic, but in reality is inert and pharmacological inactive ^(2). The placebo effect is "a change in a patient's illness attributable to the symbolic import of a treatment rather than a specific pharmacologic or physiologic property" ^(3). Note that a placebo is not even necessary to result in a placebo effect. Basically, the only way this treatment can work is if one believes that it will. No matter the fact that placebos lack actual chemical functioning, it has been documented that 30-40% of patients experience the positive effects of placebo ^(2, 5, 7). We most often hear of placebos in clinical tests of new medications or treatments, where one group in the study is given the actual medication and the other receives a placebo which feels, tastes, appears exactly like the drug. The placebo group's reactions to the placebo serve as a base-line against which the effects of the drug can be studied. In order for this drug to be considered effective, the treatment being tested much produce results better than those by the placebo. Most often, the patient's belief that the placebo will be positively effective is a self-fulfilling prophecy. A patient's belief in the treatment and the placebo response are dependent upon a variety of factors. First, the patient's expectations of treatment effects clearly influence the responses. For example, a study in England was done where 100 men were told that they were taking chemotherapy, when in actuality they were taking inactive saline solution. 20% of these men lost their hair, demonstrating the power of the belief of effects of the treatment. ^(4) Second, the relationship between patient and care-provider is quite important in determining treatment (or placebo) results. If there is a positive rapport between the two, then most often there will be a positive enthusiasm for the treatment, and therefore a positive outcome, no matter if a placebo or pharmacologic treatment was used. The friendliness, interest, sympathy, prestige, and positive attitude of the care- provider toward the patient and toward the treatment are associated with positive effects of placebos as well as of active treatments. For example, in a double- blind study of dental extractions, two groups were compared: the first, where the doctors knew they would administer either a narcotic analgesic, a placebo, or a narcotic antagonistic and the second, where the doctors knew they would administer either a placebo or narcotic antagonist. The patients from the first group who received placebo had significantly less pain. Since the two placebo groups were only different in the doctors' knowledge of possible treatment, this knowledge must have resulted in differences in behaviors by the doctor that influenced patient responses ^(3). Third, placebos have time- effect curves and peaks, cumulative and carryover effects after end of treatment which are quite similar to those of pharmacologically active medications. For example, "when varying doses of analgesic followed by a placebo are administered, patients' placebo responses correspond in degree of pain relief over time to their original dosage of analgesic" ^(3). In addition, dose-response effects have also been documented where two placebo pills were demonstrated to have more pronounced effects than one. How does one explain these trends and effectiveness of a placebo treatment? The physiology of the placebo effect is still largely unknown. A placebo is chemically inactive, yet it has a profound effect on the human mind and body. A possible explanation is that a patient's belief in the treatment will reduce anxiety by allowing the problem to seem more controllable. With regards to the example given at the beginning of the paper, you may feel much more at ease once a doctor has told you that there are effective treatments for your pain, therefore you can reap the benefits of a less-stressed state. Depression and anxiety most often increase subjective complaints of pain ^(5). Stress adversely affects several physiological processes and increases symptom reporting. Placebos seem "most effective for anxious patients and the effects are often attributed to anxiety reduction and associated decreased pain and suffering" ^(3). Thus, the patient may benefit from the relief of symptoms of depression, stress, and anxiety. In addition, the placebo does not merely allow the patient's mind to be at ease; the effect extends beyond the psyche. In a 1950 study of routine arterial ligation surgery to treat angina pectoris, 13 patients received the surgery while a placebo group of five had a chest incision made. 76% of those who received the surgery and 100% of the placebo group improved ^(7). Another explanation is the idea of conditioned stimuli. Treatment may have a positive effect because of its association with effective treatments (and interactions with the medical field) the patient has had before. Thus, inactive drugs, people, procedures, and places can function as conditioned stimuli for the alleviation of symptoms if powerful unconditioned stimuli has been positively associated repeatedly with the relief of symptoms. An additional possible explanation for the effects of placebo is that responses may be caused by endogenous opiate release in the central nervous system ^(3). Pain sensation is subject to circuitry within the nervous system that uses opioid synapses, as suggested by the fact that injection of opiates relieves pain. It is thought that possibly the placebo effect is a stimulation and release of our body's own natural narcotics, such as endorphins and enkephalins. These chemicals are typically released during stress or excitement and it is possible that they either bind to pain receptors or depress neurons in the central nervous system, thus slowing or eliminating pain communication ^(6). This shows us that, by whatever mechanism, a person's beliefs can either change his biochemistry or functionally mimic a change in biochemistry. However, studies of the exact physiological mechanism of the placebo effect are rare and difficult, especially when in most cases the use of a placebo is to test the effect of another drug. Additionally, since we know that the effect of placebos is dependent upon conditioning, faith and reduction of anxiety, the placebo response is different for everyone and cannot be necessarily predicted. Indeed, we may not know how the placebo works, yet something which makes such profound therapeutic changes must not be ignored. Many scientists believe that the effectiveness of alternative therapies may simply be a placebo effect. Yet, if the effects of treatments such as hypnosis, meditation, acupuncture, etc... remain and bring about long- lasting health benefits, then we must draw the conclusion that a placebo is more than something against which testing of a "real" drug should be done. Given the positive results of the placebo effect, how can we really say that one type of medicine or therapy is necessarily ineffective? For example, relaxation techniques generally alter sympathetic activity as indicated by decreases in oxygen consumption, respiratory and heart rate, and blood pressure. Increased electroencephalographic slow wave activity has also been reported. Although the mechanism for the decrease in sympathetic activity is unclear, one may infer that decreased arousal (due to alterations in catecholamines or other neurochemical systems) plays a key role. Hypnosis, in part because of its capacity for evoking intense relaxation, has been reported to reduce several types of pain. Finally, acupuncture has gained popularity in its ability to relieve pain, possibly due to the release of endorphins ^(5). In addition, these types of techniques may alter expectation, which also plays a key role in subjective experiences of pain intensity. They also may augment analgesic responses through behavioral conditioning. Finally, these techniques help patients enhance their sense of self control over their illness enabling them to be less helpless and better able to deal with pain sensations ^(5). If these are positive effects of alternative therapies (which the medical world claims are placebo effects), then the placebo effect must demonstrate the amazing power of our mind. Perhaps the placebo decreases anxiety. Perhaps it meets the expectations of the patient. Perhaps it is a conditioned response. Yet, the issue here is that it works. Why does much of the Western world of medicine reject a "technology" that works - the power of our minds? Most doctors shy away from the use of the word because it seems to imply a type of deception. And yet, the deception is all in our heads! "Placebo effects are recognized as an inherent part of any therapeutic intervention outcome, and demonstrate the important and power of the psychological factors in the healing process" ^(1). The placebo effect is proof of the fact that the mind can influence the body in a powerful and therapeutic way. Citations 1- Fecteau, Danielle, "Placebo Effects", Science et Comportement http://www.ovid.med.virginia.edu/ovidweb/ovidweb.cgi 2- American Cancer Society, Alternative and Complementary Therapies: http://www.cancer.org/alt_therapies/articles/placebo.html 3- Turner, et al, "The importance of placebo effects in pain treatment and research", JAMA, 1994: http://www.ovid.med. virginia.edu/ovidweb/ovidweb.cgi 4- Mining Co. Alternative Medicine Information: http://www.altmedicine.minigco.com/library/weekly/aa100998.htm?terms =placebo+eff ect&COB=home (Note: this link takes you to the Mining Co. search engine) 5- Mind/Body Medicine Discussion: http://www2.trincoll.edu/~rmlee/mindbody_lll/mb2disc_frm.htm 6- Elmer, Debra, "Placebo Use": http://www.nursing.uiowa.edu/sites/Adultpain/MedGen/placebot.htm 7- Imprint Online, "Placebo Power": http://imprint.uwaterloo.ca/issues/012398/3Science/science01.shtml Endnotes 1- "confident" literally means "with faith" (Lat: con = with; fides = faith) | Course Home Page | Back to Brain and Behavior | Back to Serendip | Send us your comments at Serendip [purplestripe.gif] (c) by Serendip 1994- - Last Modified: Monday, 07-Jan-2002 14:39:31 EST § #Collective-Evolution RSS Feed Collective-Evolution » Feed Collective-Evolution » Comments Feed Collective-Evolution » The Placebo Effect: Transforming Biology With Belief Comments Feed Italy Moves To Ban Monsanto GMO Corn Man With Stage 3 Colon Cancer Refuses Chemotherapy & Cures Himself With Vegan Diet * Home * About CE + Events + In The Media + Franco DeNicola * 1 on 1 Assistance * Store * What Can I Do? * Donate * Advertise * Contact * Home * About CE + Events + In The Media + Franco DeNicola * 1 on 1 Assistance * Store * What Can I Do? * Donate * Advertise * Contact * * * * * * Collective-Evolution CE inspires us to begin expanding our way of thinking so we can take conscious steps towards creating BIG change on the planet. CE's Mission! Join The Community! Login Register Menu * Home * Consciousness + Self-Improvement + Spirituality + Philosophy * Health + Wellness + Nutrition + Awareness * Science & Tech + Discover + Energy + Space * Alternative News + General + Environmental + Exopolitics + Political * Multimedia + Videos + Images * Blog * Discussion * Radio * Magazine * Home * Consciousness + Self-Improvement + Spirituality + Philosophy * Health + Wellness + Nutrition + Awareness * Science & Tech + Discover + Energy + Space * Alternative News + General + Environmental + Exopolitics + Political * Multimedia + Videos + Images * Blog * Discussion * Radio * Magazine * * Consciousness * The Placebo Effect: Transforming Biology With Belief The Placebo Effect: Transforming Biology With Belief July 21, 2013 by Arjun Walia. 35 Comments. (BUTTON) Share on Facebook (BUTTON) Twitter placebo Did you know that we can change our biology simply by what we believe to be true? The placebo effect is defined as the measurable, observable, or felt improvement in health or behaviour not attributable to a medication or invasive treatment that has been administered. It suggests that one can treat various ailments by using the mind to heal. For example, if two people have a head ache and one takes tylenol while the other is given a pill that contains nothing (sugar), both could report that the pill was successful and the headache is gone. The difference is, the one that was given the pill which contained nothing still believed that they were given a tylenol that would alleviate their headache. In doing so, their headache was cured because of what they believed to be true. This has happened on numerous occasions, many studies have shown that the placebo effect is real and highly effective. The placebo practice is known, but widely dismissed in medicine. The placebo effect should be a major topic of study in medical schools. Unfortunately drug companies study patients who respond to the placebo effect with the goal of eliminating them from early clinical trials. It bothers pharmaceutical manufacturers that in most of their clinical trials the placebos prove to be just as effective as their chemical ridden drugs. Examining the placebo effect would give rise to a whole new category under science, which would probably be consciousness. This is why it’s not examined thoroughly, the power of our perception and its ability to create our reality and even change our biology would open the door to a multitude of other questions, possibilities and potentialities for the human race. These potentialities would most likely wipe out many industries on the planet, from health all the way down to energy. These concepts are also heavily examined and illustrated by quantum physics. The placebo effect should be the subject of major, funded research efforts. If medical researchers could figure out how to leverage the placebo effect, they would hand doctors an efficient,energy-based, side effect-free tool to treat disease. Energy healers say they already have such tools, but I am a scientist, and I believe the more we know about sceince of the placebo, the better we’ll be able to use it in clinical settings – Bruce Lipton, Ph.D (3) A Baylor School of Medicine study, published in 2002 in the New England Journal of Medicine, (1) looked at surgery for patients with severe and debilitating knee pain. Many surgeons know there is no placebo effect in surgery, or so most of them believe. The patients were divided into three groups. The surgeons shaved the damaged cartilage in the knee of one group. For the second group they flushed out the knee joint, removing all of the material believed to be causing inflammation. Both of these processes are the standard surgeries people go through who have severe arthritic knees. The third group received a “fake” surgery, the patients were only sedated and tricked that they actually had the knee surgery. For the patients not really receiving the surgery, the doctors made the incisions and splashed salt water on the knee as they would in normal surgery. They then sewed up the incisions like the real thing and the process was complete. All three groups went through the same rehab process, and the results were astonishing. The placebo group improved just as much as the other two groups who had surgery. My skill as a surgeon had no benefit on these patients. The entire benefit of surgery for osteoarthritis of the kneww was the placebo effect – Dr. Moseley (Surgeon involved in the study)(3) Another great example of the placebo effect came from the United States Department of Health and Human Services in 1999. The report discovered that half of severely depressed patients taking drugs improve compared to the thirty-two percent taking a placebo. Don’t forget about all of the side effects and dangers that have been associated with antidepressants every year. Don’t forget that the ‘depression industry’ alone is a multi billion dollar one(3). A 2002 article published in the American Psychological Association’s prevention & treatment, by University of Connecticut psychology professor Irving Kirsch titled, “The Emperor’s New Drugs,” made some more shocking discoveries(5)(4). He found that 80 perecent of the effect of antidepressants, as measured in clinical trials, could be attributed to the placebo effect. This professor even had to file a Freedom of Information Act (FOIA) request to get information on the clinical trials of the top antidepressants. The difference between the response of the drugs and the response of the placebo was less than two points on average on this clinical scale that goes from fifty to sixty points. That’s a very small difference, that difference is clinically meaningless – Professor Kirsch Researchers all over the world have found that placebo treatments can stimulate real biological and physiological responses. Everything from changes in heart rate to blood pressure and even chemical activity in the brain. It’s been effective with a number of different ailments from arthritis, depression, fatigue, anxiety, Parkinson’s and more (2) So what does this mean? It means that through the power of belief, your biological body can react in a necessary way to target whatever ailment you are experiencing. Thoughts, feelings, and emotions are directly responsible for changing your biology. If we look at depression for example, we are told the main cause of it is a chemical deficit in the brain. But if thoughts, feelings and emotions can release different chemicals in the brain, why not just work on the patients feelings to induce a different chemical state? If our feelings, emotions and thoughts are directly correlated with our biology, why aren’t we putting more resources into this research? Why is this not practiced by the medical industry? Why do we completely turn a blind eye to it? The human race has been trained, and programmed to believe that external medicines are necessary for all ailments. I’m not saying that some medical applications are not valid, I’m just saying the human race completely ignores the power of non-physical phenomenon. We continue to believe that we need something outside of ourselves to heal, when everything points to the fact that this is not entirely true. Our own biological system and the human being is very capable of healing itself. We just don’t know how, we don’t believe it, we are not exposed to it. Changing your biology with belief is not an easy process, because most of us don’t truly believe we can. We are going up against years of perceptual manipulation that have formed our thoughts and beliefs. Your beliefs shape your perception, and your perception is what creates real phenomenon. If you change the way you perceive things, the things you perceive change. We are powerful beings, and have abilities that have yet to be unlocked. I believe that these types of realities will continue to emerge and will be implemented in the future. The placebo effect demonstrates, from a biological standpoint, that what you believe indeed becomes your reality. For one to be able to use this, they must believe it. One must perceive it as real as the perceive their own hand real, the sun real, the stars real. It’s not about believing, it’s about knowing. The true nature of reality has yet to be discovered, but we continue to progress in our understanding. As we progress we realize how obsolete our current way of functioning really is. It’s time to evolve past our archaic ideas and false beliefs, and step into a new understanding of reality. We are capable of so much more, or potential is limitless. Sources: (1)http://www.nejm.org/doi/full/10.1056/NEJMoa013259 (2)http://harvardmagazine.com/2013/01/the-placebo-phenomenon (3)Lipton, Bruce. The Biology of Belief. Hay House, Inc, 2005 (4)http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=mind-reviews-the-em perors-new-drugs (5)http://psycnet.apa.org/?&fa=main.doiLanding&doi=10.1037/1522-3736.5. 1.523a email (BUTTON) Share on Facebook (BUTTON) Share on Twitter About the author Arjun Walia arjun@collective-evolution.com My Articles More From 'Consciousness' MeditationsforEnlightenment Enlightenment: Is It Really What We Think It Is? January 2, 2014 by Elina St-Onge IMG_5770 Creating From The Heart To Manifest Your Dreams December 26, 2013 by Emily Millett LifeA This Revolutionary Transformative Tool Can Change Your Life December 18, 2013 by Collective Evolution Anna_Breytenbach What If Animals Could Talk To You? Fascinating Woman Speaks Telepathically With Animals. December 17, 2013 by Collective Evolution DON'T MISS OUT ON NEW CE CONTENT! CE provides a space for free thinkers to explore and discuss new, alternative information and ideas. The goal? Question everything, think differently, spread love and live a joy filled life. RSS ____________________ (BUTTON) Subscribe Discuss with other free thinkers Discuss with other free thinkers 35 comments on “The Placebo Effect: Transforming Biology With Belief” 1. darkmoon December 2, 2013 at 4:42 am what about migraine ? is it just all psychologically driven and can a placebo also help alleviate migraine pain? Reply 2. RAHEEM REMTULLA November 7, 2013 at 10:10 pm Im done with this………………………… Reply 3. Pingback: Efeito Placebo: Transformando a sua biologia através da convicção. « METANOIAN 4. Pingback: Thou mind’s power to heal – The Placebo Effect | Rajkamal Mohanram's Blog 5. Pingback: The Progressive Mind » Natural Health News and Wellness Tips | Natural Remedies and Products: MCT’s In Coconut Oil Treat Alzheimer’s and Boost Cognition In One Dose 6. Pingback: Forbidden News » MCT’s In Coconut Oil Treat Alzheimer’s & Boost Cognition In One Dose 7. Pingback: Coconut Oil Proven to Help Treat Alzheimer’s & Boost Cognition | Collective-Evolution 8. Dave Kool August 14, 2013 at 12:25 am There was a similar study done where some basketball players were divided into three groups: one that would practice free throws for an hour a day, one who would only visualize practicing free throws for an hour a day, and the final group did basically nothing. Predictably the group that did nothing showed no improvement on the number of baskets they made, the group that practiced showed the most improvement, but interestingly enough, the group who only VISUALIZED practicing their free throws showed a marked improvement, almost as much as the group who was actually practicing. Just goes to show that perception and visualization are nearly as important as actions themselves. Reply + Rich August 15, 2013 at 11:09 am In all kindness in addition: A ‘study’ is also the creating of a reality version. (“We are not the cool observers of the world, but its “passionate creators” — we are all poets and the world is our metaphor.” – Roger S. Jones) After all it is a possibility in the dream state of consciousness to to create and experience a similar experience of being real. Science aka ‘knowledge’, ‘science fiction’ is a product of imagining. Reply + Rich August 15, 2013 at 2:40 pm In addition and in all kindness. “I see no ships European explorers found indigenous peoples unable to see their tallships – or did they? By David Hambling February 2007 Are people blind to unexplained phenomena because their brains simply can’t handle anything they don’t understand? This story, quoted in social science circles and popular with New Agers, was repeated in the recent film What The Bleep Do We Know? – “When the tall European ships first approached the early Native Americans, it was such an ‘impossible’ vision in their reality that their highly filtered perceptions couldn’t register what was happening, and they literally failed to ‘see’ the ships.” A more detailed account, “allegedly found in a diary in Magellan’s own handwriting”, 2 describes how South Americans could see the boats that the explorers landed in, but not the ships anchored offshore. [...]” http://tinyurl.com/ybjbzbe & “Gautama Buddha [...] We are what we think. All that we are arises with our thoughts. With our thoughts we make the world. [...]” http://tinyurl.com/lae32pt & “Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia 1. Peter Gordon Members of the Pirahã tribe use a “one-two-many” system of counting. I ask whether speakers of this innumerate language can appreciate larger numerosities without the benefit of words to encode them. This addresses the classic Whorfian question about whether language can determine thought [...]”. http://tinyurl.com/mwuujnc The information which, for one , is used to (co-) create with, is aka ‘language’. According to ‘Ethnologue’ there are about 7,105 languages. http://tinyurl.com/m6cbev8 These 7,105 languages consist out of a multitude of concepts (information). Being ignorant of most all of these concepts also means that (co-) creating realities using all these concepts is impossible. No information for ‘tall ships’ also means no focusing with this information, and no (co-) creating of a reality with ‘tall ships’. This also applies to the ‘holographic illusion’ know as ‘New York’ , or ‘histories’ or what ever. (Reality is merely an illusion, albeit a very persistent one. – Albert Einstein -(Adjusted version: Reality is merely an illusion, albeit a very persistent holographic one. )) “We are what we think.” But we can only use the tools we have. When those ’tools’ aka ‘languages/concepts’ are lacking, they cannot be used to (co-) create realities with. When those ’tools’ aka ‘languages/concepts’ are alternatives, (co-) created alternative realties manifest. Numerical Cognition is using ‘languages/concepts’ to (co-) create with. When these ‘language/concepts’ are lacking (being ignorant of these concepts), (co-) creating with these ‘languages/concepts’ is impossible. Ignorance is bliss and is most common! Being ignorant of the information regarding an activity and to take or have a part in or share this activity, is impossible. Even if according to ‘language/concepts’ it is an ‘illness’, a ‘sport’ or ‘whatever’. In all cases ‘energy’ cannot assume holographic form manifestations, if the information (for one ‘languages/concepts) in use to focus with, is lacking. Regarding holograms: “Japanese scientists create touchable holograms.” For one: http://tinyurl.com/2996nyw Is Our Universe a Hologram? In 1982 a Litttle Known but Epic Event Occured at the University of Paris (Today’s Most Popular) For one: http://tinyurl.com/6e5nlu4 *Definition: “Everything is a holographic energy form variation manifestation, within consciousness, by which energy assumes the holographic form variation and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to the information* which is in use to focus with and in doing so co-create with.” Reply o Rich August 15, 2013 at 7:59 pm Motto: Errors fail to become by means of propagation in multitude. Verify everything – The Buddha. Thus the following corrections; Questioning perceptual blindness I see no ships http://tinyurl.com/ybjbzbe Gautama Buddha http://tinyurl.com/lae32pt Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia http://tinyurl.com/3pg4ncv Is Our Universe a Hologram? In 1982 a Litttle Known but Epic Event Occured at the University of Paris (Today’s Most Popular) http://tinyurl.com/6e5nlu4 The information which, for one , is used to (co-) create with, is aka ‘language’. According to ‘Ethnologue’ there are about 7,105 languages. http://tinyurl.com/m6cbev8 Japanese scientists create touchable holograms. http://tinyurl.com/2996nyw Is Our Universe a Hologram? In 1982 a Litttle Known but Epic Event Occured at the University of Paris (Today’s Most Popular) http://tinyurl.com/6e5nlu4 Reply 9. Pingback: Re-Write Reality. | ThirdiRadio 10. Pingback: The Placebo Effect: Transforming Biology With Belief | Phoenix Tree Productions 11. Will van Tienen August 3, 2013 at 4:25 pm There is no difference between energy and material! It’s all the same principle: Religion, Mantra, Belief, Mindfullness, The Secret. You create your own reality. You just have to believe it yourself! Reply + Rich August 5, 2013 at 4:31 am Kindly in addition: Energy is conscious, holographic, formless, transformable. Focusing information and energy assumes holographic forms which manifest. Language and cultural codes: “How does relativity theory resolve the Twin Paradox?” Ronald C. Lasky, a lecturer at Dartmouth College’s Thayer School of Engineering, explains. Time must never be thought of as pre-existing in any sense; it is a manufactured quantity. –Hermann Bondi..” [...] http://tinyurl.com/llggv9m Altering the information which is in use to focus with, also alters the manifestation accordingly. In regard to the “Twin Paradox” substituting the persons, for one, known as ‘The Twins’ with persons, for one known as ‘Mother and her child’ and the manifestation is accordingly. The person ‘the mother’ , being the person who travels with the rocket, and the person ‘the child’ who remains on earth, at a certain point according to the information know as the “Twin Paradox/Mother and Child Paradox” are ‘equally old’ and at another point ‘the mother is younger than her own child’ and ‘the child is older than it’s own mother’. Consciousness: “A honours degree in mathematics with “no detectable brain”? Consciousness The predominant view amongst neuroscientists is that human consciousness is a product of the electrical activity of the brain. They call it an “emergent property” of grey matter. The basic assumption there is that if you take approximately 100 billion nerve cells and you wire them together through a prodigious network of connection, consciousness somehow “emerges” spontaneously from that complexity. One neuron is not conscious, but 100 billion, taken together, are. This materialist theory of consciousness is up against some formidable challenges, all happily ignored by the mainstream materialist neuroscientists. [...] A brain is also a holographic manifestation with in consciousness, a possibility but not a necessity. “Grey Walter’s Anticipatory Tortoises” lack a brain manifestation altogether, yet have consciousness. http://tinyurl.com/q8q42lh Focusing information: Jacko deemed to be a gorilla of Amsterdam Zoo Artis, in percentages, is doing ‘remarkably’ well buying and selling shares for years. Those persons who are deemed to be Jacko’s caretakers however are the beings who are focusing the information and believe Jacko’s share picks/instructions. http://tinyurl.com/l4aq47o Altering information and thus altering possible beliefs: There is no such thing as a “Nobel Prize In Economics”. There is “The Sveriges Riksbank Prize in Economic Sciences in Memory of Alfred Nobel”. A prize of the central bank of Sweden. http://tinyurl.com/ldf2724 http://tinyurl.com/l4z46e5 It is however possible to use language and pretend there is a “Nobel Prize In Economics”. http://tinyurl.com/88f76fv Energy doesn’t judge, it just assumes holographic forms which manifest and can be propagated as beliefs. http://tinyurl.com/88f76fv Reply o Rich August 6, 2013 at 10:01 pm Link for “…Consciousness: “A honours degree in mathematics with “no detectable brain”?…” Link: http://tinyurl.com/kgqyhpv Reply # Rich August 9, 2013 at 7:36 am The principle of the definition* also applies for the conceptualization known as: “…self-fulfilling prophecy Any positive or negative expectation about circumstances, events, or people that may affect a person’s behavior toward them in a manner that causes those expectations to be fulfilled. An employer who, for example, expects the employees to be disloyal and shirkers, will likely treat them in a way that will elicit the very response he or she expects. …” http://tinyurl.com/kb4s3as “…Self-fulfilling prophecy From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia A self-fulfilling prophecy is a prediction that directly or indirectly causes itself to become true, by the very terms of the prophecy itself, due to positive feedback between belief and behavior. Although examples of such prophecies can be found in literature as far back as ancient Greece and ancient India, it is 20th-century sociologist Robert K. Merton who is credited with coining the expression “self-fulfilling prophecy” and formalizing its structure and consequences. In his book Social Theory and Social Structure, Merton defines self-fulfilling prophecy in the following terms: e.g. when Roxanna falsely believes her marriage will fail, her fears of such failure actually cause the marriage to fail. …” http://tinyurl.com/kyxvmty *Definition: “Everything is a holographic energy form variation manifestation, within consciousness, by which energy assumes the holographic form variation and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to the information* which is in use to focus with and in doing so co-create with.” Reply @ Rich August 9, 2013 at 3:58 pm “Obese? Smoker? No Retirement Savings? Perhaps It’s Because of the Language You Speak Why can’t the Greeks be more like the Germans? Could it be because they speak Greek? There’s no doubt some nations save more money than others, and plan better for retirement, and watch their collective weight, but the proposed explanations for different levels of future-mindedness have been historical, sociological, cultural and psychological—not grammatical. In this ingenious paper (pdf), though, M. Keith Chen argues that syntax plays a role. His analysis suggests that if your language’s syntax blurs the difference between today and tomorrow (as do, say, Chinese and German) then you are more likely to save money, ….” http://tinyurl.com/9aqaose & “In New Quantum Experiment, Effect Happens Before Cause A real-world demonstration of a thought experiment conducted at the University of Vienna, has produced a result that is somewhat befuddling to people with what the lead researcher calls a “naïve classical world view.” Two pairs of particles are either quantum-entangled or not. One person makes the decision as to whether to entangle them or not, and another pair of people measure the particles to see whether they’re entangled or not. The head-scratcher is: the measurement is made before the decision is made, and it is accurate. “Classical correlations can be decided after they are measured,” says Xiao-song Ma, the writer of the study. Entanglement can be created “after the entangled particles have been measured and may no longer exist.” …” http://tinyurl.com/7b7dj3u Albert Einstein’s ‘Spukhafte Fernwirkung’ is about instantaneous manifestation. There is neither cause nor effect. Real-Time Imaging of Quantum Entanglement YouTube http://tinyurl.com/n5mehrs However altering the information (languages for one) which is in use to focus and create with will also have energy assume forms accordingly which manifest as a reality. Using the information ‘effect happens before cause’ is also possibly applicable for concepts know as “virgin birth”. http://tinyurl.com/8926j33 o Rich August 11, 2013 at 2:35 pm “Splitting (also called all-or-nothing thinking) is the failure in a person’s thinking to bring together both positive and negative qualities of the self and others into a cohesive, realistic whole. It is a common defense mechanism used by many people.[1] The individual tends to think in extremes (i.e., an individual’s actions and motivations are all good or all bad with no middle ground.)….” http://tinyurl.com/d6czkxz Albert Schweitzer, M.D. on the Doctor Within Every Patient “Within every patient there resides a doctor, and we as physicians are at our best when we we put our patients in touch with the doctor inside themselves.” http://tinyurl.com/65goswb The language (information) in use by Mr Albert Schweitzer is ‘black & white’ . This ‘school of thought’ is for one relative to the languages (information) known as the “Abrahamic religions” (Judaism, Christianity, Islam, and Bahá’í Faith.) http://tinyurl.com/6wjsvhf And by shifting the focus from being a creator (co-creator) to the being an observer, a “Wild Goose Chase” manifests. Mr Albert Schweitzer not only (co-) creates the ‘Doctor Within’, but also the ‘disease within’, in his reality manifestation; but fails to recognize that creating (co-creating) is at play, not observing. Energy just assumes forms-no matter what. Reply # Rich August 14, 2013 at 1:01 am Charles Robert Darwin’s ‘On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life.’ (The Origin of Species.) http://tinyurl.com/noxdjfa “[...]WHEN we look to the individuals[...]” The, for one, phenomenon Mr Charles R. Darwin’s focus is a shifting from being a creator (co-creator) to being an observer. (“We are not the cool observers of the world, but its “passionate creators” — we are all poets and the world is our metaphor.” – Roger S. Jones) The, for one, phenomenon Mr Charles R. Darwin fails to account for and integrate into ‘The Origin of Species’ the implications of, for instance: ‘the blind spot’, ‘filtering of the senses’, ‘the language concepts and conceptualizations (English -black/white-deductive) in use’. But does focus subconsciously, using the tools he does have, incomplete as they are, information, and energy just assumes holographic forms which manifest. Of course being ignorant of English and thus also of ‘The Origin of Species’ beliefs, also has energy remain formless, in this regard. Reply 12. Tahir C August 2, 2013 at 4:48 am Okay, so there’s no disagreement that humans are capable of doing much more. But there’s also a huge tendency of mixing up reality with illusion, and a very fine line separates out both from each other. I don’t want to get all hasty on getting quotes from all those famous personalities. If this was the case then Homeopathy had been doing this thing for a long time, and if it were that successful regarding placebo then we would not even have Allopathy today. Not even this, the ancient medicines like herbal medicines etc would have been more than enough. Why move to even Homeopathy or even Allopathy then? Why was there Polio vaccination, hmm? And then there’s a long list of ailments which ancient medicines utterly failed to cure and were treated only by Allopathy successfully. There’s certain objectivity in placebo, but it has been used to prove delusion most of the times. There used to be a time when humans “believed so hard” that “they could have wings”, and many even tried to fly by attaching fake wings, “believing hardly” that their belief would make those fake wings a “real ones” (i.e. shaping their perception); and then they could easily proceed to fly in air. Sadly, this lead to countless deaths in the human history. And even if that would have been the case then we would not be having airplanes among us today. It shall be so easy then just to “believe” that one has wings, and then everybody would be flying easily. Also, it would be rather simple just to believe that one is full and would not proceed to eat something. And we see doing that would rather lead to starvation, which is harmful on another level. And the list simple goes on. But hey, then there are further arguments on “believing rightly” and this would lead to infinite arguments for the sake of arguments, as in all this has happened because “belief” was not carried out rightly or a belief was not believed correctly; and this would further aggravate things up as in which one is the “right” way of believing, further leading into infinite dialogues. We can see countless religions a result of “believing rightly”. A belief is merely a very subjective thing. I would rather say the opposite, to that of what the write has mentioned, and that is perceptions shapes beliefs. A belief in its very nature is purely subjective. We have made other material tools because we need them. For instance an eye is a tool to see things and we need it to see light. If we would have been able to see light without eyes, then why do we have eyes in the very first place? Same goes for our arms, legs, body and other material tools. But let us not forget that humans need tools, which are our extensions. Let us not forget that one has to see the picture in totality, which would eventually take us to a certain objectivity. And that objectivity will be purely without subjectivities. Reply + Rich August 2, 2013 at 12:54 pm Reality is an illusion. The blind spot test is one way to become aware. A reality illusion of a complete computer screen is thought to be seen, but the eyes have blind spots so the information is already incomplete. The missing information is added. See: Online Eye Test: Find Your Blind Spot in Each Eye! – http://tinyurl.com/l9znwgh The senses filter. Reply o Predrag Lozanovski August 3, 2013 at 10:15 am @Tahir, I think you’re confusing the areas where belief does or doesn’t work. For example, you’ll never turn into an elephant no matter how hard you believe to be one. But you will very well overcome an ailment – or at least alleviate its symptoms or pains and/or stimulate your recovery. Yet the most important part – which again is most difficult to prove or demostrate – is the fact that by means of belief you can always prevent yourself from becoming ill, especially from those new and mostly preconceived “ailments” like arising of cancer as a result of smoking. So, belief works with all those things that exist as a result of our BELIEVING IN THEIR EXISTENCE. Sure you can also change some things within this “Reality” that seem impossible to change, like flying for instance. But to do that, you’ve got to alter some parts of “Reality” as well – as it’s the case in your dreams. You can’t fly as long as you accept all those physical laws that are congruent parts of our “Reality”. As long as you believe in those physical laws, you’re bound to the earth. The problem seems to be our BELIEVING. Insects like May-beetles for instance, DON’T believe in physical laws, that’s why they’re able to fly. If they believed in those physical laws, they’d “know” that their bodies are not shaped aero-dynamically, so they would never even try to fly. So, if you want to go further than just overcome some problems that are easy to solve – like not becoming ill of something, Millions of people get ill from – you’ve got to alter your own system of beliefs, thusby sort of producing “your own Reality”. If you do that, you’ll realize that some things will become possible for you, and other will not. And then you’ll realize that your own intervention within this widely accepted “Reality” is limmited. As a final result you’ll probably find out that your ultimate success should be TO CREATE YOUR OWN REALITY that is BASICALLY DIFFERENT THAN THIS ABNORMAL ONE, where we exist as long as we believe in it the way it is. In other words, you’ll be able to overcome this “Reality” when you stop believing that it was created by God or is an unchangable course of Evolution. Your final and ultimate developent starts when you realize that “God” or “Evolution” are NOT EXTERN, but WITHIN YOU. You are the Creator of your own NEW AND INDIVIDUAL Reality and partly an executor within this OLD AND COLLECTIVE “Reality”. :) Reply 13. johnsonwi10 July 29, 2013 at 11:57 pm Wow, can it really be possible? From Statistics class lectures, it said that the placebo effect worked more than half the time, but I didn’t know it was this serious. I wonder if I can finally end my depression and pain from simply believing. Why aren’t we funding research on this? The potentialities are LIMITLESS!!! Or are we funding this? Reply + Rich July 30, 2013 at 2:49 am Why aren’t we funding research on this? In all kindness: Everything which is needed, you already have! Use (focus with) information which creates the end-result of that which you do want only, in the now. A) ”Reality is merely an illusion, albeit a very persistent one.” – Albert Einstein http://tinyurl.com/5kkgm & Are We Living in a Holographic Universe? This May Be the Greatest Revolution of the 21st Century http://tinyurl.com/6b7wdlf Adjusting Albert Einstein’s quote: “Reality is merely an illusion, albeit a very persistent holographic one” & B) Man sieht nur das, was man weiß. You only see what you know. (One only experiences what one knows). Johann Wolfgang von Goethe http://tinyurl.com/mvk4c2d & Bible: New International Version (©2011) In the beginning was the Word, and the Word was with God, and the Word was God. http://tinyurl.com/p7vg5xs That which one ‘knows’, in general, is a multitude of concepts, for one, known as a language. Aka ‘the word’. Use concepts (words/language/information) and energy assumes holographic form variations and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to the information. Energy is free from judging, cultural codes etc. etc. Energy just assume holographic form variations which manifest as realities. See: Numerical cognition without words: evidence from Amazonia”. In: Science Express, 10.1126/science.1094492 (2004) “…Members of the Pirahã tribe use a “one-two-many” system of counting. I ask whether speakers of this innumerate language can appreciate larger numerosities without the benefit of words to encode them. This addresses the classic Whorfian question about whether language can determine thought. Results of numerical tasks with varying cognitive demands show that numerical cognition is clearly affected by the lack of a counting system in the language. …” http://tinyurl.com/mwuujnc The persons who are deemed to be the Pirahã tribe are also ‘co-creators’ and use their concepts. Energy still just assumes holographic form variations and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to this information. The holographic reflection of the persons who are deemed to be the Pirahã tribe manifests as “one-two-many”. Four, five, six doesn’t manifest as this information is lacking and cannot be used to focus with. Information can also be used to create a “Wild goose chase”. Shifting the focus via deduction and using ‘word camouflage”.le An exemple The persons who are producing are ‘producers’. The persons who are consuming are ‘consumers’. Statement: “The producers are optimistic and the consumers are pessimistic.” The concept ‘person’ is deducted and the Wild Goose chase is created. After all a person who produces also consumes and should the statement belong to one’s belief system, a person is both ‘optimistic’ and ‘pessimistic’, simultaneously. Energy still just assume holographic form variations which manifest as realities, with in consciousness. 1) In the awake state of consciousness it is possible to experience walking down a beach, meeting people, see reflections in the water etc.. 2) In the dream state of consciousness it is possible to experience walking down a beach, meeting people, see reflections in the water etc.. 3) In the imagining state of consciousness it is possible to experience walking down a beach, meeting people, see reflections in the water etc.. All these alternative states of consciousness feel ‘real’ and form realities. Yet from the dream state of consciousness we know, only we personally can be the co-creator of the reality-hologram (illusion). And it is still energy which assumes holographic form variations and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to the information. It is all ‘in the mind’. Reply + Predrag Lozanovski August 5, 2013 at 6:41 pm @johnsonwi10: Ending your depression and pain by simply believing should be the easy part, any good Psychologist could help you at it. It need not even be a Psychologist, any gifted person could do it as well. The crucial part is your reaction. If you believe, anything within the sphere of believing is possible. Let me tell you this: If a doctor or a priest tell you that you’re ill or sinfull, you can’t but BELIEVE THEM, so you’re ill or sinfull, because you believe that you are. When that doctor or priest tells you that you’re cured or your sins are forgiven, you BELIEVE THEM again, since you can’t find it out yourself, so you’re cured or forgiven because YOU BELIEVE IT. So, belief works within the sphere of Authorities that we put our trust in – doctors, scientists, priests, “God”… Knowing that and the fact that your body is best equipped with every thing that is neccessery to make you ill or to heal you, you all of a sudden realize that EVERY THING DEPENDS ON YOU AND YOUR BELIEVING. This works even if you believe in some higher powers – like God for instance. When you’re in a trouble or ill from a dangerous ailment you can believe that God can help you – and if you believe it hard enough, YOU WILL be cured. As a matter of fact you’ll ALWAYS get healed by YOUR OWN BELIEVING, not because an external instance has helped you, but it’s still up to you to define WHO helped you. If you’re very young you will probably believe that God had helped you. If you’re old enough to understand that YOU ALONE ARE ALMOST TOTALLY SUFFICIENT TO MAKE YOURSELF ILL OR TO HEAL YOURSELF – you’ll understand that IT WAS YOU who helped you. Of course there is one more option that you can choose to believe in, although it’s pretty questionable and far from being proven by anyone. That option involves your FUTURE “ME”. It is possible that at a future moment we all will be able to intervene in our own Past, in order to save ourselves from some personal disasters. Maybe only some people will be able to do that, but it doesn’t matter now, the point is that it is possible for you in future to act back in time in order to save yourself from something. You’re probably able to remind yourself of some situations when you evaded a danger without knowing how or why that happened. As for me, I’ve been in several situations when I got rescued from something in a way that leaves the option open for my future ME to have intervened to my favour – other options however not excluded. Back to your question: Depression and pain are no real obstacles for your belief. You can handle them just like that. Although I’m not a believer, let me remind you of Jesus’ words: “If you have faith the size of a small mustard seed, you’ll say to this mountain ‘Move over!’ – and it will obey.” I’m not referring to the religious faith in God. I’m referring to FAITH IN GENERAL, since religious faith is just one form of FAITH or BELIEVING. So one of the “things” you’ve got to do is believe that things are not as they seem. What we see is what WE HAVE BEEN TAUGHT TO BE TRUE. Some of it corresponds with Reality, while many things are far from being real – and yet we believe in them. So if you ask me, I’d advice you to first of all realize that YOU ARE THE MOST IMPORTANT “THING” IN THE WHOLE “UNIVERSE” – which will make it easy for you to develop your faith or your self-confidance. That as a start. Every thing else will follow as a normal and logical advancement of your personal growth. Not saying that every thing will be ok – but in spite of all your future problems – or yet better: THANKS TO THEM you’ll become more and more effective, gradually overcoming this present so-called “Reality” and upgrading your own personal Reality. Reply 14. Rich July 26, 2013 at 1:47 am INVENTOR OF ADHD’S DEATHBED CONFESSION: “ADHD IS A FICTITIOUS DISEASE” http://tinyurl.com/b3y7uvc Not only does the placebo effect work to create a cure but also to create the disease. There is nothing either good or bad but thinking makes it so. William Shakespeare http://tinyurl.com/4fom4jc Of course, in order to think ‘what ever’ one needs the concepts. It be ‘good’, ‘bad’, ‘disease’, ‘cure’, ‘ADHD’, etc. No concepts and energy will not assume a form and remain formless. Kindly refer to the definition. Reply 15. Rich July 25, 2013 at 8:03 am I don’t believe in mathematics. Quoted by Carl Seelig. Albert Einstein. http://tinyurl.com/kbxm5z4 Albert Einstein negates/nullifies mathematics; he is not solving mathematical problems, but creating the problems and the solutions. After all, mathematics is also a holographic energy form variation manifestation, within consciousness. Albert Einstein e=mc^2 is about ‘energy’. It is not about ‘light-speed’. Light-speed is not energy, but also a holographic energy form variation manifestation. Yet using the information ‘light-speed’ to focus with will have energy act according the the definition. Light-speed is also a ‘belief’ as is the ‘placebo-effect’. It is impossible for energy to go ‘faster’ than itself. There is no need to either! Please see definition. Definition: “Everything is a holographic energy form variation manifestation, within consciousness, by which energy assumes the holographic form variation and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to the information* which is in use to focus with and in doing so co-create with.” Reply 16. Rich July 24, 2013 at 3:10 am Is Economics a Science or a Religion? Bloomberg – http://tinyurl.com/oalfl5g Economics is also information which is ‘believed’ as are all other sciences, academic and otherwise, and energy will plain and simply assume holographic form variations which will manifest as a reality. Reply 17. Rich July 24, 2013 at 1:53 am A definition which one can learn by heart. After all, one can read all one wants, go to lectures all one wants, forgetting, is no matter what, a human quality. Definition: “Everything is a holographic energy form variation manifestation, within consciousness, by which energy assumes the holographic form variation and manifests as a holographic reflection relative to the information* which is in use to focus with and in doing so co-create with.” *Information is factor X. Change the information and energy assumes an alternative form variation which manifests relative to the alternative information. “We are not the cool observers of the world, but its “passionate creators” — we are all poets and the world is our metaphor.” – Roger S. Jones Measurement Problem – Reality is an Illusion YouTube http://tinyurl.com/cglc5ve In this YouTube video replace ‘observer’ with the person who co-creates by focusing information, and the puzzle will fall into place. Reality is holographic, but English, for one, is lacking the holographic quality of energy in the concepts which form the English language. By focusing information even if it is lacking the holographic quality still has energy act according to the definition above. Reply 18. Rich July 23, 2013 at 2:48 am The following url gives a visualisation of how information has energy assume form variations. Water, Consciousness & Intent: Dr. Masaru Emoto https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=tAvzsjcBtx8 Reply 19. Predrag Lozanovski July 22, 2013 at 2:18 pm GREAT ARTICLE! (I knew about those facts long time ago, my favourite notions sound: “our body is the most powerful factory” and “the way it (an eilment) comes, the way it goes”. I haven’t been seriously ill for over 35 years. During that time I’ve had only 3 tooth extractions and a chirurgical intervention on hemorrhoides.) Reply 20. Jeremy July 22, 2013 at 12:05 pm Well, I dont know of some of the things that bother me, only that I experience pain in a certain area and it has been diagnosed, after almost a year and a million dollars woth of tests and drugs as “I dont know what ails you!” So I meditate on a daily basis that my body is healthy and will stay that way… Reply 21. Rod Dalitz July 21, 2013 at 5:01 pm This is also the case for homeopathy. Reply 22. waltinseattle July 21, 2013 at 4:18 pm “hyperactive children who are given a “dose extender” IN FULL KNOWLEDGE THAT it is an INACTIVE PILL ” can reduce their regular meficstion by half with no ill effects” from Slights of Mind by Strphen Macknik and Susana Martinez-Conde…neuroscientists both…pg 210 btw there is no mind brain body separatness. all one “Thing”..a thing that include “process” as well as “objects.” Reply Leave a Reply Cancel reply IFRAME: jetpack_remote_comment A quick note before submitting a comment... Collective Evolution welcomes differing viewpoints and thought-provoking opinions that add value to the discussion, but comments may be moderated to remove profanity or remarks that detract from a healthy conversation. For the best interest of the community, please refrain from posting vulgar comments, profanity, or personal attacks. Comments submitted will be reviewed by our moderation team before appearing on the website. ____________________ * 9,038 Followers * 323,859 Fans * 1,477 Followers * Subscribe RSS/Feed IFRAME: https://madmimi.com/signups/76397/iframe_embed [square_300x250.jpg] www.amazers.org Subscribe to CE Magazine Monthly For Exclusive Content! We Recommend www.truththeory.com www.amazers.org Collective Evolution (CE) creates content that engages us all to begin thinking consciously about what it means to be a human on the planet. New releases, updates, sneak peeks & exclusive deals right in your inbox. ____________________ (BUTTON) Subscribe Circa 2009 --Who Needs Copyright? Powered by a Bunch of Crisp Kids | Privacy Policy | DMCA | § #The Verge - All Posts The Verge via www.casascius.com Trending now Why don't economists like Bitcoin? * The incandescent light bulb isn't dead * A veteran with PTSD was the first to purchase legal marijuana in Colorado * Sasmung's Galaxy Camera 2 (hands-on) * Welcome to the Age of the Upgrade Facebook Twitter Google+ YouTube Subscribe to The Verge via RSS 8 New articles back next January 03, 2014 * Samsung chairman urges staff to innovate 'non-stop' and... * DreamWorks will release its own 'Dreamtab' Android tablet... * 'Veronica Mars' trailer shows what $5.7 million in... January 02, 2014 * Gorgeous glass sculptures let you see into the world's most... * CBS and NBC to stream NFL playoff coverage for free * Snapchat to offer security fix in the wake of leaked user... * NSA secretly funding code-breaking quantum computer... * Hollywood tallies up new box office record in 2013, led by... * Polaroid announces its own 4K TV, prices it at only $999.99 * Is DNA analysis stuck in the past? * Intel killed its internet TV project because it couldn't... * Surface Pro 2 now shipping with faster processor, just two... * BlackBerry and singer Alicia Keys part ways * Watch the intricate dance of objects levitated by sound * Street lamps transform Montreal into a living comic book * A quantum theory of Netflix's genre tags * Moto G coming to Verizon Wireless on January 9th for $99.99... * The lost secrets of webOS * The story of 'Reservoir Dogs' retold in over 1,000 tweets * Kanye West-themed Bitcoin clone said to launch this month * ZTE's 2014 lineup includes 'Iconic Phablet' and a hotspot... * Samsung's Galaxy Camera 2 starts to refine the Android... * 'Unmanned' director Robert Greenwald on how America... * Samsung Shape speakers target Sonos for multiroom wireless... * LG's webOS TV revealed in leaked image * Ford's C-Max Solar Energi concept car is a sun-powered... * Walt Mossberg and Kara Swisher launch Re/code news site,... January 01, 2014 * Skype Twitter account hacked, anti-Microsoft status... * The bitter truth about making a natural sugar substitute * Skydivers share their view from above the clouds in... * Amazon is now charging sales tax in Indiana, Nevada, and... * Beyoncé sample of Challenger disaster prompts NASA response * The incandescent light bulb isn't dead * Electro destroys Times Square in New Year's Eve 'Amazing... * Moto X now permanently priced at $399 without contract * New additions to Netflix streaming include 'Spaceballs' and... * A war veteran with PTSD is the first person to buy legal... * Dubai rings in the new year with largest fireworks show ever * '100' dares you to film yourself learning a new trick in a... * Alleged Snapchat hackers explain how and why they leaked... * Federal judge upholds the government's right to search... * New York City could replace iconic horse carriages with... * Hubble telescope finds evidence of clouds on a distant... * 4.6 million Snapchat phone numbers and usernames leaked * Google-acquired Bump shutting down this month * Default-avatar + Log in or join * Longform * Video * Reviews * Tech * Science * Culture * Design * Business * US & World * Forums * Back to top * Search Search articles & pr (BUTTON) Go Loading * Previous Story BlackBerry bringing BBM to Android and iOS this summer * Next Story BlackBerry announces BBM 'Channels' social platform for brands and celebrities * Apps * Science * Report Could an app replicate the placebo effect? New research shows deception isn't needed to induce healing effects with fake meds By Adrianne Jeffries on May 14, 2013 11:00 am @adrjeffries 81Comments Don't miss stories Follow The Verge Follow medical cupcakes Daniel Jacobs believes in the placebo effect, the well-documented but not well-understood phenomenon in which sick patients sometimes feel the same healing effects from swallowing a sugar pill that they would from taking a real one. He believes in it so strongly that he doesn’t believe people even need to take a pill in order to produce a positive placebo effect; an app with a picture of a pill will do. That’s the basis of his startup, Placebo Effect, which is raising $50,000 through the crowdfunding site Indiegogo to build his prototype into an iPhone app that he says can harness the placebo effect in order to help people make positive changes in their lives, such as feeling happier or quitting smoking. __________________________________________________________________ "New research shows deception may not be necessary for the placebo effect" The app offers a variety of "placebos," including images of a pill, a magic wand, a communion wafer, and other options. "Placebo pills are actually chosen often. About 12 percent of people in our testing choose pills," he said. "The reason for that probably is that in our society, we feel that pills work really well." He’s done limited testing with good results, he said, and plans to do more. So far, 39 people self-reported an average of 31 percent increase in the effect they were trying to create in their lives, for example joy, energy, physical healing, or love, after one use. Seven users reported no change, and one person reported a negative change and did not complete the trial. Jacobs’ idea may sound a bit bogus, especially since it is widely believed that the placebo effect only works if the patient believes he or she is taking a real treatment. This perceived need for deception is part of the reason doctors don’t prescribe placebos, despite the fact that they can occasionally work as well as FDA-approved treatments for some conditions. IFRAME: http://www.youtube.com/embed/3J73TEnPj5w However, new research suggests deception may not be necessary to induce a placebo effect. In 2010, researchers from Harvard Medical School and other institutions did a study on patients diagnosed with Irritable Bowel Syndrome (IBS). One group received no treatment, while the other patients were told they’d be taking inactive drugs and told that placebos can have healing effects. To drive the point home, the fake drugs were placed in bottles labeled "placebo pills." The patients who knew they were taking placebo pills reported twice as much relief as the control group. The placebo had healing effects comparable to some of the best real IBS drugs. "Our results challenge ‘the conventional wisdom’ that placebo effects require ‘intentional ignorance,’" the researchers wrote. "Placebos are used to test treatments, but in the future they could be the treatment" If placebos could be effectively prescribed without the need to lie to patients, it would be revolutionary. Placebos are cheaper and safer than many treatments. Studies have shown placebos to be 75 percent as effective as antidepressants. Fake surgeries have been shown to be just as effective as real surgeries in treating Parkinson’s. Today placebos are used to test treatments, but in the future they could be the treatment. Some doctors are already prescribing placebos this way. "We should be using every tool in the box," Ted Kaptchuk, a Harvard professor, leading placebo effect researcher, and alternative medicine expert, told Harvard Magazine. Kaptchuk worked on the IBS study that showed a placebo effect even when patients knew they were taking a placebo. He now runs the Program in Placebo Studies and the Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS) at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, a group dedicated to placebo research. The placebo effect varies from person to person, and some may be genetically predisposed to it. The type of placebo and the condition also matter: pills work better for insomnia, studies have found, while injections work better for pain. It's also important not to cause the nocebo effect, in which patients suffer the same harmful side effects associated with a real medication, for example, even when taking the fake version. It works like the placebo effect but induces negative reactions instead of positive ones. "Placebo" translates to "I will please" in Latin. "Nocebo" translates to "I will harm." Kaptchuk and his colleagues have plenty of critics within the FDA, the pharmaceutical industry, and academia. A group of Danish researchers conducted a meta-analysis in 2001 and again in 2004, concluding that there is "little evidence in general that placebos had powerful clinical effects." It’s also unclear how much of the measured effects are due to the placebo effect, and how much might be due to other factors. It’s been suggested that patients experiencing improvement could be have a reporting bias, for example, that skews in favor of the result they believe will please their doctors. ""This is extremely effective."" As to whether the placebo effect can be simulated by an app, more research is needed there too. (Due to the lack of empirical evidence, Kaptchuk declined to comment, via his program manager, on whether an app could induce the placebo effect.) "What we’re not saying here is, ‘stop taking your antidepressants and to then do this instead,’" Jacobs said. "What we are saying is that the literature shows, and our own internal studies, and later on, clinical trials that we will surely do, that this is extremely effective. It would be wonderful if you could complement this with whatever else you’re doing in life." * Image Credit Clever Cupcakes (Flickr) * Related Items health daniel jacobs indiegogo crowdfunding placebo effect nocebo effect ted kaptchuk program in placebo studies and the therapeutic encounter Headlines * via cdn2.sbnation.com Samsung chairman urges staff to innovate 'non-stop' and drop hardware focus * DreamWorks Dreamtab DreamWorks will release its own 'Dreamtab' Android tablet this spring * Veronica Mars 'Veronica Mars' trailer shows what $5.7 million in crowdfunding buys you * Tom Brady / Patrios (KEITH ALLISON - FLICKR) CBS and NBC to stream NFL playoff coverage for free * snapchat stories 2 (ellis) Snapchat to offer security fix in the wake of leaked user data Latest Media * Gallery Ev71_best_small Luke Jerram 'Glass Microbiology' photos about 9 hours ago * Gallery Dsc_0427-1024-1024_small Samsung Galaxy Camera 2 hands-on pictures about 18 hours ago * Video VMS the verge mobile show The Verge Mobile Show 074 - December 31st, 2013 3 days ago 1 comments * Gallery apollo set 9 NASA's Apollo photography 11 days ago 2 comments * Video Mac Pro 2013 review still Mac Pro 2013 review 11 days ago 5 comments Discussions * Off-topic / chit-chat What do you think about our idea? about 1 hour ago by valentinno * Microsoft Tribe 8" Windows Tablets - Buy now or wait for CES? about 2 hours ago by Firipu * Off-topic / chit-chat How Many Devices Do You Use? about 2 hours ago by v5point0 5 comments * Microsoft Tribe Surface tablets selling better than nexus tablets in USA and Canada. about 4 hours ago by harshpwd 5 comments * Apple Core Best Windows 8/8.1 VM Solution? about 5 hours ago by grouperfish 2 comments * Microsoft Tribe Google Apps on Win RT about 6 hours ago by yanic 6 comments * Microsoft Tribe I want my own email....suggestions please. about 8 hours ago by hemi79 14 comments * Gaming Game recommendations for 360? about 8 hours ago by michael.klepacki 1 comment * Meta Is The Verge The "Official" News Site for CES 2014? about 9 hours ago by NPfeifer 3 comments * Apple Core If Apple isn't innovating then who is? about 10 hours ago by Leroysboy 60 comments All Search articles & pr (BUTTON) Go Ethics Statement - Community Guidelines Terms of Use - Privacy Policy v1.9.1, September 30, 2013 All Systems Operational Check out our status page for more details. The Sections * Longform * Video * Reviews * Tech * Science * Culture * Design * Business * US & World The Basics * Site Status * About * Contact * Tip Us * Forums * Jobs * Mobile Site The Elsewhere * Facebook * Twitter * Google+ * Tumblr * YouTube * Instagram * Subscribe Vox Media © 2014 Vox Media, Inc. All rights reserved. Designed in collaboration with Code and Theory. X The Verge Polygon SB Nation Log In Sign Up T F G Y O use Yahoo! or OpenID ____________________ Open ID Log in ____________________ ____________________ forgot? [ ] Remember me? Log in Log In Sign Up T F G Y O use Yahoo! or OpenID ____________________ Open ID Log in or ____________________ ____________________ ____________________ Continue Forgot password? We'll email you a reset link. If you signed up using a 3rd party account like Facebook or Twitter, please login with it instead. ____________________ Submit Forgot password? Try another email? Almost done, Register Spinner Authenticating Great! Choose an available username to complete sign up. ____________________ Sign Up In order to provide our users with a better overall experience, we ask for more information from Facebook when using it to login so that we can learn more about our audience and provide you with the best possible experience. We do not store specific user data and the sharing of it is not required to login with Facebook. (*) Yes, share my data ( ) Please don't share my data Submit Quantcast [audmeasure.gif?liveconclientID=5497559777280&PageID=650423876&EventTyp e=view] § LiveScience MENU Search FOLLOW * Follow on Facebook * Follow on Twitter * Add Google+ Circles * Space * Animals * Health * Planet Earth * Tech * Human Nature * History * Strange News * Video * Image Albums * Infographics * Countdowns * View Full Site 11 Surprising Facts About Placebos Joseph Brownstein, LiveScience Contributor Date: 03 June 2013 Time: 08:00 AM ET IFRAME: //www.facebook.com/plugins/like.php?href=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.livescience.c om%2F37073-surprising-facts-placebo-effect.html&send=false&layout=butto n_count&width=450&show_faces=false&font&colorscheme=light&action=like&h eight=21&appId=438608976204523 Tweet * Introduction Intro CREDIT: Brian Hoskins | Stock Xchng Pills * Early placebo research was done in the 18th century for consumer protection. A woman holds two aspirin in her hand. 11 CREDIT: Pills photo via Shutterstock aspirin-pills-water-120910 * Even real treatments benefit from the placebo effect. A bottle of prescription pills 10 CREDIT: Prescription photo via Shutterstock prescription-drug-130328 * "Placebo" and "the placebo effect" refer to different things. 9 Assorted Pharmaceutical Drugs Credit: Dreamstime * There is a "nocebo" effect. A woman holds her aching head 8 CREDIT: Headache photo via Shutterstock woman-headache-130415 * Many clinical trials no longer include placebos. doctor's office, appointment, physical 7 CREDIT: Doctor's visit photo via Shutterstock Doctor's visit * Placebos have a long-term, demonstrated effect in relieving pain. fibromyalgia 6 CREDIT: Yuri Arcurs | shutterstock fibromyalgia, neck pain Tender points on the neck are one sign of fibromyalgia * Scientists don't know if certain groups of people are more susceptible to placebos. A bottle of pills 5 CREDIT: Pills photo via Shutterstock pill-bottle-120619 * The placebo effect is actually several effects. pills-natural-101014-02 4 CREDIT: Alita Bobrov | Dreamstime Pills Natural * Psychiatric illnesses are among the conditions most susceptible to placebos. brain cells, neurons 3 CREDIT: Dreamstime Brain The brain is made up of an inconceivable number of cells. * Not all placebos are created equal. drugs 2 CREDIT: Shutterstock.com drugs Drugs, when taken in combination, can cause severe side effects. * Placebo does not equal being duped. 1 FDA Issues Serious Reality Check on Diet Pills Photo taken by Steve Knight (stevekrh19). There are no usage restrictions for this photo The placebo effect plays a central role in doctors' understanding of whether medications work. The effects of a placebo, or "dummy," treatment cannot come from the actual treatment itself. In other words, the placebo effect explains why people report feeling better when they think they are taking medicine, but aren't actually receiving the treatment. (The word placebo comes from the Latin for "I will please.") The placebo effect can help explain why drugs and other treatments with no value can seem to work, and therefore, why some treatments pass muster while others do not. However, the placebo effect itself remains mysterious. Doctors still have questions about how it operates, and why its effect can be large, small or nonexistent for a given treatment or patient. The following pages share some of the history, knowledge and misconceptions about the placebo effect. IFRAME: //www.facebook.com/plugins/like.php?href=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.livescience.c om%2F37073-surprising-facts-placebo-effect.html&send=false&layout=butto n_count&width=450&show_faces=false&font&colorscheme=light&action=like&h eight=21&appId=438608976204523 Tweet Most Popular * Home * About Us * Desktop site Follow us RSS Facebook Twitter YouTube Join our Mailing List Enter your email add (BUTTON) [footer-logo.png] Copyright . All rights reserved. § Book T of C Chap T of C Prev page Next page space holder ad [psiteeny.gif] Placebo Effects Believing in something often helps to make it work. This is called the placebo effect. A placebo (pronounced pluh-SEE-bo) is literally a "pleasing thing." The word usually refers to a sugar pill or fake medicine. However, the phrase "placebo effect" has come to mean much more. To most psychology researchers, the phrase placebo effect now refers to any situation in which a person's belief in a treatment causes the treatment to work (Critelli & Neumann, 1984). What are placebo effects? Are they imaginary? Placebo effects, defined this way, are not imaginary. They are genuine changes produced by a person's knowledge or belief in a manipulation. For example, if you wore a certain cologne which made you feel more attractive, you might act more attractive and be more attractive. This would be a placebo effect if your belief affected your behavior, but the cologne by itself had no real effect. To determine whether that was the case, the placebo effect would have to be controlled. How do you control a placebo effect? When testing the effects of a new medicine, researchers must give the control group a placebo-a realistic-looking "fake" which contains no active ingredient. The experimental group gets the real medicine, the control group gets the placebo. Both groups think they are getting a real medicine. This way the researchers can tell if the medicine has any effect beyond the expected placebo effect, which should be present equally in both groups. Students have no trouble understanding this idea, yet many still miss this question: 1) How do you control the placebo effect? a) give the control group an experimental treatment b) give the experimental group a "sugar pill" c) create a placebo effect in the control group d) make sure nobody gets a placebo The correct answer is "c." To some students that sounds wrong. If it sounds wrong to you, study this concept until you understand it. A researcher tries to create a placebo effect in the control group to equalize the placebo effect in the two groups. That removes the placebo effect as a confounded variable, because it is not a difference between groups. (In other words, both groups must believe they are receiving a genuine treatment, to equalize the placebo effect and remove it as a confounding variable.) What is a single-blind design? An experimental design in which subjects do not know whether they are receiving a placebo treatment or a real treatment is called a single-blind design. The subjects are blind to which treatment they are receiving, although the experimenter may be aware of which they are getting. Prev page | T of C | Next page __________________________________________________________________ Write to Dr. Dewey at psywww@gmail.com. __________________________________________________________________ Don't see what you need? Psych Web has over 1,000 pages, so it may be elsewhere on the site. Do a site-specific Google search using the box below. __________________________________________________ Search Google Custom Search Copyright (c) 2007 Russ Dewey § #RSS Feed IFRAME: http://www.facebook.com/plugins/like.php?href=tvtropes.org%2Fpmwiki%2Fp mwiki.php%2FMain%2FHomePage&layout=button_count&show_faces=true&width=1 60&action=like&colorscheme=light&height=21 Nabla troperville * Welcome To TV Tropes * Ask The Tropers * Forum * Trope Repair Shop * Recent Discussions * Latest Reviews * You Know, That Thing Where... * Remember That Show? * Lost And Found * Administrivia * Works That Need A Summary * What Goes Where Nabla tools Request a content violation evaluation. (BUTTON) send request __________________________________________________________________ * Page Type: usefulnote set page type __________________________________________________________________ Find recent edits by troper: ________ (BUTTON) find __________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________ This article needs some moderator assistance with: _______________ _______________ Please don't bother to do this without saying what the issue is in the box above. No issue, no action. send report ______________________________________________________________ * Title Search * Customize Wikiword * Cut List * Trope Repair Shop * New Edits * Edit Reasons * Recently New * Launches * Get Known * Your Profile * Add Email Address * Indexes * Image Uploader * Images List * Referral Count for This Page * Crowner Activity * Folderize * Un-typed Pages * Recent Page Type Changes __________________________________________________________________ * Editing Overview * Good Style * Rule Of Cautious Editing Judgment * Text Formatting * Page Templates * Wiki Sandbox __________________________________________________________________ Nablatoys * Pitch Generator * Story Generator [ott_sai_final-167.png] SubpagesMain __________________________________________________________________ main index Narrative * Universal * Applied Phlebotinum * Characterization * Characters * Characters As Device * Dialogue * Motifs * Narrative Devices * Paratext * Plots * Settings * Spectacle Genre * Action Adventure * Comedy * Commercials * Crime And Punishment * Drama * Horror * Love * News * Professional Wrestling * Speculative Fiction * Sports Story * War Media * Animation (Western) * Anime * Comic Book * Fan Fics * Film * Game * Literature * Music And Sound Effects * New Media * Print Media * Radio * Sequential Art * Tabletop Games * Television * Theater * Videogame * Webcomics Topical Tropes * Betrayal * Censorship * Combat * Death * Family * Fate And Prophecy * Food * Holiday * Memory * Money * Morality * Politics * Religion * School Other Categories * British Telly * The Contributors * Creator Speak * Creators * Derivative Works * Language * Laws And Formulas * Show Business * Split Personality * Stock Room * Trope * Truth And Lies * Truth In Television TV Tropes Org _______________________________ search new edits workshops reviews YKTTW tweets random Subpages: [____] source log in history edit page related * Discussion * Analysis Placebo Effect Sheldon: Do you believe in the placebo effect? Amy: Of course I do. There have been many studies proving its validity. Sheldon: Great. Now, this may look like a Tic Tac, but it is really a powerful medication specifically designed to cure your illness as well as freshen your breath. -- The Big Bang Theory The placebo effect is where a patient thinks that a 'medicine' is healing them, even though it doesn't have an actual medical effect. The most common use is in drug trials, in which a control group is given a placebo, to compare the effects in case the drug actually is only effective due to the placebo effect, or is even worse. One difference between fiction and reality is that, unlike in fiction, real placebos often continue to have an effect even if the user finds out it's a placebo - the action of taking the "drug" is usually enough to fool the body. The opposite is the nocebo effect, in which someone believes something is hurting them or making them sick, when it's really not. This could even kill them, as Your Mind Makes It Real. See Magic Feather in terms of the plot. A more realistic version of Clap Your Hands If You Believe. __________________________________________________________________ Examples: open/close all folders Anime and Manga * One chapter of Sayonara, Zetsubou-Sensei has a field day with this trope, including Nozomu taking a placebo drug and breaking out in hives from the percieved side effects, Stalker with a Crush Matoi forcing Nozomu to take a fake Love Pill, only for the effect to work on her instead when she's the only one convinced that it worked, and Nozomu eventually getting fake surgery performed on him to get rid of the fake side effects from the fake drug he took at the start, and ending up convinced that he's been turned into an android. + The chapter also throws some Leaning on the Fourth Wall into the mix, by introducing a sticker that, if you stick it to a manga, makes it six times funnier. Immediately the sticker starts showing up more and more, until the last two pages are covered in them. Film * In Eurotrip, two of the characters order brownies at an Amsterdam bakery run by a Dreadlock Rasta. They immediately become stoned beyond belief...until the Dreadlock Rasta calmly informs them that they're not hash brownies. * The protagonist of Matchstick Men, who has severe OCD, is given a packet of pills by his psychiatrist that apparently heals him. Around the same time, he starts bonding with his long-lost daughter. He eventually learns that the pills are just soy menopause supplements, and that bonding with his daughter has given his life meaning and helped him overcome his neurosis. * The Birdcage: Armand's "Pirin tablets" -- he seems to think they're some kind of powerful anti-anxiety medication; they are in fact Aspirin with two letters scraped off. They seem to do the job, though. * In SpaceJam, the Tune Squad has completely given up hope of winning their basketball game against the Monstars until Bugs Bunny gives them 'Michael's Secret Stuff', which buffs them up and gives them the confidence to get back in the game. Of course, the real secret is that it is just water. * Subverted in The Exorcist. In one scene, a priest douses the demon-posessed protagonist with tap water and claims that it's holy water, but the protagonist screams in pain anyway. Later, though, it's implied that the demon only pretended to be fooled so that the priests would think that the "possession" was purely psychological and wouldn't try to exorcise it. When the priests douse the protagonist with real holy water in the climactic exorcism scene, the screams are real and the water leaves visible burns. * An inadvertent psychotherapy version appears in What About Bob?: Bob's new therapist gives him a copy of his book Baby Steps, which is about setting small, attainable goals. Bob completely misses the point and thinks that he has to physically take small steps wherever he goes. However, Bob's focus on taking "baby steps" when he gets nervous helps distract him from his fears and allows him to do things he wasn't able to do before. Literature * In It, it is revealed that Eddie Kaspbrak's asthma is psychosomatic, and his medication is a placebo. * Inverted by Captain Underpants, who thinks his powers are neutralized if he's sprayed with spray starch. Live Action TV * Star Trek: The Original Series At the end of Mudd's Women. * The Red Green Show had an episode where the lodge members were part of a test-market for an energy bar, making them very active and becoming addicted. When the test batch ran out, they reverted back to normal, only for Harold to reveal all they got was a basic granola bar to gauge product interest, due to the real stuff being too dangerous. Red, taking this as proof of the strength of his mind, salvages one of the test sample bars and tells his wife at home to wait up that night. * In an episode of Frasier, Niles eats a normal brownie thinking it's a pot brownie, with the reverse situation for Martin. Hilarity Ensues. * One time on Mash they run out of morphine so they give the patients sugar pills telling them it's a strong painkiller. It works. + On the B-plot of that episode, they're experiencing a heat wave that has everyone miserable. They give Klinger some of the sugar pills claiming they're some sort of new drug that will allow him to feel cooler. He spends the rest of the episode in his regular uniform while everyone else is wearing undershirts. * The Suite Life on Deck: Bailey uses a placebo to raise London's intelligence. Subverted in that after realizing that it's a placebo, London returns to normal. Then she takes another placebo. * On The Big Bang Theory, Raj is unable to talk to women unless he's drunk. In "The Terminator Decoupling," the guys are on a train when they discover that Summer Glau is in the same car, and they all try to hit on her. Raj drinks copious amounts of beer before going over to talk with her, and she actually seems to like him. Then Howard walks over and informs him that it was non-alcoholic beer. He clams up and walks away without another word. * Referred to when the MythBusters tested seasickness cures. To be certain that Adam and Grant weren't subconsciously skewing the test results, Jamie gave each of them an "over-the-counter medicine" that was actually a vitamin pill. (Grant fell for the placebo, but Adam got sick just as fast as in the other tests.) * In The Next Mutation, the Turtles have a recurring team of foes that once used a magic formula to enhance their abilities. The effect ended when the turtles pointed out the 'magic' was just a placebo. * Used when NewsRadio parodied Flowers for Algernon. Joe makes Matthew a "Smart Drink", which works until Matthew gets smart enough to realise it's only working because he thinks it will. Matthew: It's like when Wile E Coyote runs off a cliff. He can stay standing in mid-air until he looks down. * Penn And Tellers Bullshit episode "Yoga, Tantric Sex, Etc." featured a man trying to explain why herbs work for some people and not for others. Penn summed up the entire argument in one sentence. Penn: If you believe they work they work, if you don't they don't. You hardly ever hear that about penicillin. Western Animation * Dogbert has a placebo that works even when the user knows it's a placebo. + That, and the people he's giving them to are usually too stupid to realize what a placebo is. * The Simpsons: The Crazy Cat Lady once regained her sanity thanks to a medication but lost it when she learned it was a placebo. * In the Handy Manny segment "Fearless Rusty", Lovable Coward Rusty is sprinkled with "magic dust" by a local magician, who claimed it would make him brave... and he believes it. * In one episode of Hey Arnold!, Helga bought an anti-love potion that successfully killed her feelings for Arnold only to learn later on that it was just grape juice. Real Life * Chiropractic adjustments are controversial, owing to the fact that there's little scientific proof that the person doing an adjustment (technically a Doctor of Chiropractic, since they aren't actually medical doctors) is doing anything other than basic decompression of the spine. But because these 'doctors' are very good at convincing people they'll feel better, they do tend to feel better. * Acupuncture also has very little proof that it is effective (yes, they are able to do studies with needles that look like they are inserted, but actually are not), but people really believe that it helps them. * Therapeutic Touch therapy and it's "Eastern" equivalent, reiki, worked through this effect as well - it's so easily debunked that a 9-year old girl was able to do it, as seen here [external_link.gif] . * Homeopathy may fool many people due to the placebo effect, despite the fact that it would have to utterly violate some of the most basic laws of physics and chemistry in order to work. * Iridology. [external_link.gif] In the words of The Other Wiki: Iris texture is a phenotypical feature which develops during gestation and remains unchanged after birth. * Pretty much all pseudoscientific "treatments" depend on this. They are mostly (not entirely, but mostly) directed toward the relief of (chronic) pain, which is one of the most incredibly subjective things on the planet to attempt to measure. * Guess why the pharmaceutical industry spends more money on advertising than research. [external_link.gif] __________________________________________________________________ Pitch Generator More Like a Footnote than Anything Else Raekuul Dump new edits workshops edit page random TV Tropes by TV Tropes Foundation, LLC is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 3.0 Unported License. Permissions beyond the scope of this license may be available from thestaff@tvtropes.org. Privacy Policy 19956 5 § #assertTrue( ) - Atom assertTrue( ) - RSS assertTrue( ) - Atom assertTrue( ) "I can explain." by Kas Thomas Sunday, March 24, 2013 New Patents Aim to Reduce Placebo Effect The pharma industry has a big problem on its hands: Placebos are getting to be way too effective. Something needs to be done. But what? What can you do about placebo response? The old saying "It is what it is" would seem to hold true in this case. One answer: come up with low-placebo-response study designs, and patent them if possible. (And yes, it is possible. But we're getting ahead of the story.) Placebo effect has always been a problem for drug companies, but it's especially a problem for low-efficacy drugs (psych meds, in particular). An example of the problem is provided by Eli Lilly. In a March 29, 2009 press release announcing the failure of Phase II trials involving a new atypical antipsychotic known as LY2140023 monohydrate or mGlu2/3, Lilly said: In Study HBBI, neither LY2140023 monohydrate, nor the comparator molecule olanzapine [Zyprexa], known to be more effective than placebo, separated from placebo. In this particular study, Lilly observed a greater-than-expected placebo response, which was approximately double that historically seen in schizophrenia clinical trials. [emphasis added] [PlaceboDoctor.png] Fast-forward to August 2012: Lilly throws in the towel on mGlu2/3. According to a report in Genetic Engineering and Technology News, "Independent futility analysis concluded H8Y-MC-HBBN, the second of Lilly's two pivotal Phase III studies, was unlikely to be positive in its primary efficacy endpoint if enrolled to completion." Lilly is not alone. Rexahn Pharmaceuticals, in November 2011, issued a press release about disappointing Phase IIb trials of a new antidepressant, Serdaxin, saying: "Results from the study did not demonstrate Serdaxin's efficacy compared to placebo measured by the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS). All groups showed an approximate 14 point improvement in the protocol defined primary endpoint of MADRS." In March 2012, AstraZeneca threw in the towel on an adjunctive antidepressant, TC-5214, after the drug failed to beat placebo in Phase III trials. A news account put the cost of the failure at half a billion dollars. In December 2011, shares of BioSante Pharmaceutical Inc. slid 77% in a single session after the company's experimental gel for promoting libido in postmenopausal women failed to perform well against placebo in late-stage trials. The drug companies say these failures are happening not because their drugs are ineffective, but because placebos have recently become more effective in clinical trials. (For evidence on increasing placebo effectiveness, see yesterday's post, where I showed a graph of placebo efficacy in antidepressant trials over a 20-year period.) Some idea of the desperation felt by drug companies can be glimpsed in this slideshow (alternate link here) by Anastasia Ivanova of the Department of Biostatistics, UNC at Chapel Hill, which discusses tactics for mitigating high placebo response. The Final Solution? Something called The Sequential Parallel Comparison Design. SPCD is a cascading (multi-phase) protocol design. In the canonical two-phase version, you start with a larger-than-usual group of placebo subjects relative to non-placebo subjects. In phase one, you run the trial as usual, but at the end, placebo non-responders are randomized into a second phase of the study (which, like the first phase, uses a placebo control arm and a study arm). SPCD differs from the usual "placebo run-in" design in that it doesn't actually eliminate placebo responders from the overall study. Instead, it keeps their results, so that when the phase-two placebo group's data are added in, they effectively dilute the higher phase-one placebo results. The assumption, of course, is that placebo non-responders will be non-responsive to placebo in phase two after having been identified as non-responders in phase one. In industry argot, there will be carry-over of (non)effect from placebo phase one to placebo phase two. [PlaceboSPCDProtocol.png] This bit of chicanery (I don't know what else to call it) seems pointless until you do the math. The Ivanova slideshow explains it in some detail, but basically, if you optimize the ratio of placebo to study-arm subjects properly, you end up increasing the overall power of the study while keeping placebo response minimized. This translates to big bucks for pharma companies, who strive mightily to keep the cost of drug trials down by enrolling only as many subjects as might be needed to give the study the desired power. In other words, maximizing study power per enrollee is key. And SPCD does that. SPCD was first introduced in the literature in a paper by Fava et al., Psychother Psychosom. 2003 May-Jun;72(3):115-27, with the interesting title "The problem of the placebo response in clinical trials for psychiatric disorders: culprits, possible remedies, and a novel study design approach." The title is interesting in that it paints placebo response as an evil (complete with cuplrits). In this paper, Maurizio Fava and his colleagues point to possible causes of increasing placebo response that have been considered by others ("diagnostic misclassification, issues concerning inclusion/exclusion criteria, outcome measures' lack of sensitivity to change, measurement errors, poor quality of data entry and verification, waxing and waning of the natural course of illness, regression toward the mean phenomenon, patient and clinician expectations about the trial, study design issues, non-specific therapeutic effects, and high attrition"), glossing over the most obvious possibility, which is that paid research subjects (for-hire "volunteers"), who are desperate, in many cases, to obtain free medical care, are only too willing to tell researchers whatever they want to hear about whatever useless palliative is given them. But then Fava and his coauthors make the baffling statement: "Thus far, there has been no attempt to develop new study designs aimed at reducing the placebo effect." They go on to present SPCD as a more or less revolutionary advance in the quest to quelch placebo effect. Up until this point in science, I don't think there had ever been any discussion, in a scientific paper, of a need to attack placebo effect as something bothersome, something that interferes with scientific progress, something that needs to be guarded against vigilantly like Swine Flu. The whole idea that placebo effect is getting in the way of producing meaningful results is repugnant, I think, to anyone with scientific training. What's even more repugnant, however, is that Fava's group didn't stop with a mere paper in Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. They went on to apply for, and obtain, U.S. patents on SPCD (on behalf of The General Hospital Corporation of Boston). The relevant U.S. patent numbers are 7,647,235; 7,840,419; 7,983,936; 8,145,504; 8,145,505, and 8,219,41, the most recent of which was granted July 2012. You can look them up on Google Patents. The patents begin with the statement: "A method and system for performing a clinical trial having a reduced placebo effect is disclosed." Incredibly, the whole point of the invention is to mitigate (if not actually defeat) the placebo effect. I don't know if anybody else sees this as disturbing. To me it's repulsive. If you're interested in licensing the patents, RCT Logic will be happy to talk to you about it. Download their white paper and slides. Or just visit the website. Have antidepressants and other drugs now become so miserably ineffective, so hopelessly useless in clinical trials, that we need to redesign our scientific protocols in such a way as to defeat placebo effect? Are we now to view placebo effect as something that needs to be made to go away by protocol-fudging? If so, it puts us in a new scientific era indeed. But that's where we are, apparently. Welcome to the new world of wonder drugs. And pass the Tic-Tacs. Posted by Kas Thomas at 12:30 AM Email ThisBlogThis!Share to TwitterShare to Facebook 14 comments: 1. [anon36.png] Stephen Senn4:02 PM The statisticians involved, at least, ought to know better. 1.They are assuming that placebo response is genuine and not just regression to the mean (the latter is usually grossly underestimated). 2. they are assuming that it is reproducible and not transient. 3. they are assuming that the FDA is asleep. If the Agency is not dozing they will make them market the drug as being only suitable for those who have a proven inability to respond to placebo. That should sort them out. ReplyDelete 2. [anon36.png] Anonymous8:53 PM Could you expand please, on why it is repugnant to attempt to lower the placebo response? I've heard this before, and I'm genuinely interested as to why this is a problem. I see it as a signal-detection issue where solving it may have consequences for generalizability etc. I don't really see it as a moral issue, but there's every chance I'm missing something? As an aside, it strikes me that best design to test a new drug would be one where *no-one* knows they are receiving it. Completely unethical of course, but we could be guaranteed that any observed effect (positive and negative) was actually due to biologically induced change - rather than expectation, demand characteristics etc. Thus, eliminating the placebo effect would be extremely useful here, if we care about knowing the true harms and benefits of the drug in question. P.s., I'm not psych meds qualify as low-efficacy treatments. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22297588 Paul ReplyDelete 3. [anon36.png] Stephen Senn6:46 AM Paul, if you take the point of view that the practical question is to see how a drug improves the situation beyond what would normally happen then it seems appropriate to have as a control what would normally happen. What is labelled placebo response is in any case in most cases just regression to the mean. I regard all this placebo response stuff as just so much junk science. See http://eprints.gla.ac.uk/8107/1/id8107.pdf and also Kienle, G. S. and H. Kiene (1997). "The powerful placebo effect: fact or fiction?" Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 50(12): 1311-1318. ReplyDelete 4. [anon36.png] Anonymous7:58 AM Stephen, thanks for your reply and the links. I agree with your point about pragmatism but I also see the value of a carefully controlled assessment of efficacy & harms - not least to avoid Type I error for the former and Type II for the latter. I think there must be room for both approaches... P.s are you the Stephen Senn of CROS analysis fame? Paul ReplyDelete 5. [anon36.png] Paul Ivsin12:00 PM I also am failing to fully grasp the ethical problems of this study design, and of attempting to reduce the placebo response in RCTs in general. Presumably, reductions in placebo response would happen across all arms of the trial equally, which would provide a clearer picture of the specific chemical activity of the drug. The major problem with the design seems to be -- as Stephen points out in the first comment -- that it probably won't reduce the total placebo effect size much. If placebo responders were likely to remain consistent in their response, then we'd already have a simple solution in the placebo run-in period. The problem being, of course, that there's really no evidence that run-in periods do what they're supposed to do. I actually wrote a brief blog post about the SPCD a couple years ago (here). Then, I asked essentially the same question as now: what evidence to we have that SPCD reduces placebo response compared to more traditional methods? (and a second question for the author: if SPCD is an unethical design, then wouldn't a design featuring a placebo run-in -- which most trials use -- also be unethical?) ReplyDelete 6. [anon36.png] Anonymous12:52 PM The problem is that there are some people in both treatment and control groups who are placebo-responders -- they will respond no matter what you give them. Responders therefore have to be kept in the control group as the proper baseline for comparison. If they're left out in any way, you're comparing apples and oranges -- a treatment group that includes some unknown placebo responders (whose response to the drug is therefore inflated) is being compared to a control group that excludes placebo responders. Right? ReplyDelete 7. [anon36.png] Jim10:32 PM I think the issues here are a little more complicated than presented by the author. We have a simple observation, that drugs that robustly separated from placebo 20 years ago no longer reliably do so. Did they become less active? Probably not. Are placebos working better than they used to? Probably, at least within the clinical trial setting. Assuming that is the case, what should we do? 1)We could remove all these drugs from the market, and simply give patients placebos. But its not really clear whether the placebo effect would continue to operate once word got out that doctors were routinely handing out placebo. In fact once study found that the magnitude of the placebo effect is proportional to the likelihood of receiving active drug. 2)We could leave the old drugs on the market and not approve the new ones. However the new drugs appear to be equally efficacious to the old ones, and in many cases (e.g., lurasidone) have better side effect profiles. This would seem not to serve the interests of patients either. 3)We could attempt to make clinical trials look more like real life treatment, in which the choices are not between drug and placebo but between drug and no treatment. But this would lead to drugs with no intrinsic efficacy being approved as a matter of course. 4)We can try to understand the placebo effect, and engineer it out of trials so that we know that approved drugs have greater efficacy that placebo under at least some test conditions. I would say that choice #4 is probably the best of those that I can come up with. For all the insinuations in the article about how pharma is trying to pull the wool over everyone's eyes, it seems that given the available choices, the industry is pursuing the only one that really makes any sense. Obviously we would all like to have more efficacious drugs whose effects are so robust that the issue of establishing separation from placebo never arises. If the author has any ideas on how such compounds might be produced, I'm sure there are venture capitalists who would be strongly interested to hear them. ReplyDelete 8. [anon36.png] Anonymous7:40 AM Jim, very interesting points. Something that doesn't seem to be getting enough consideration from the industry is that improvements in trial methodology (governance, oversight, pre-registration, CONSORT etc) has reduced risk of bias, both in relation to trial conduct and reporting, and that this might account for some of the increased placebo effect. I wonder what your view on this is? If I am right, then it follows that older trials overestimated efficacy. Paul ReplyDelete 9. [anon36.png] Anonymous7:39 AM to Jim: you are assuming that the only option is to treat patients with drugs(be it placebo or "real" effective drugs). However, at least in case of depression, psychotherapy can be highly effective: take cognitive behavioural therapy as good example. There are even options for self-therapy(look at the excellent book by CBT therapist David Burns "Feeling Good: the new mood therapy"). So no, the industry isn't "pursuing the only one that really makes any sense." ReplyDelete 10. [anon36.png] Anonymous9:39 AM I remember having read a very enlightening piece about why the old drugs don't work these days: they never did, and the reason is really simple: the industry would only publish studies that showed a positive effect and hide those that showed a negative effect. Bad study design and reporting bias does the rest. You always get studies with a positive outcome, even if the drug itself does not work. So any study design that tries to lower placebo response is a great idea for the industry, because that is a cost-effective way of creating more studies with positive outcome, independently of the efficay of the drug. Now tell me, what's not to love about that? Bernard ReplyDelete 11. [anon36.png] mendelid11:22 AM Statistically, I can see arguments both ways. Pro SPCD: If we assume that a psychiatric diagnosis describes a symptom rather than an underlying cause, and that placebo is efficient at counteracting one cause and the medication efficient at counteracting another, and both are equally strong and independent of each other, then we would get the same number of positive responses for each medication, yet it would still make sense to have that medication. The SPCD would in this case show that the medication is efficient for causes where the placebo isn't. An ineffective medication that only works through the placebo effect should still fail SPCD studies. (An additional argument pro SPCD is that due to the low efficacy of psychiatric drugs, practitioners test patients on one drug, and if it doesn't work, switch them to another one. SPCD mimics this practice by switching patients on whom the placebo didn't work to the medication being tested. Thus it could be said to be more relevant to actual medical practice.) Contra SPCD: The above argument assumes that the placebo effect works the same, no matter if it's the medication or the actual placebo that causes it. However, that may not be true. In that case, the SPCD may simply funnel a higher percentage of subjects into the second phase who are able to detect placebos (e.g. through missing side effects). For these people, the placebo effect of the real drug may be stronger, perhaps because they're noticing side effects that weren't present in the previous placebo trial, so they (correctly) conclude they're now given "the real thing", making the placebo effect work for them. ReplyDelete 12. [anon36.png] Anonymous11:09 AM Didn't you jump the gun with this article, by about a week? "The drug companies say these failures are happening not because their drugs are ineffective, but because placebos have recently become more effective in clinical trials." Hahahaa!!! But wait, the date says March 24! ReplyDelete 13. [anon36.png] greentess1:09 PM Thank you for sharing. The ViS platform enables clinical research optimization. ReplyDelete 14. [anon36.png] Earn Money Clinical Trials8:26 PM Why it has to be reduced if it is getting to its way in being effective at all? ReplyDelete Add comment Load more... Please add your comment here! Newer Post Older Post Home Subscribe to: Post Comments (Atom) Subscribe To This Blog! [arrow_dropdown.gif] Posts [subscribe-netvibes.png] [subscribe-yahoo.png] [icon_feed12.png] Atom [arrow_dropdown.gif] Posts [arrow_dropdown.gif] Comments [subscribe-netvibes.png] [subscribe-yahoo.png] [icon_feed12.png] Atom [arrow_dropdown.gif] Comments Sometimes it's okay to be a Follower: Translate Yours truly My Photo Kas Thomas Blogger at BigThink.com in addition to here. Trend-sniffer-outer. Iconoclast. Humanist. Degrees in biology and microbiology. For more, see me on LinkedIn. View my complete profile Blog Archive * 2013 (205) + December (21) + November (10) + October (9) + September (12) + August (20) + July (13) + June (9) + May (14) + April (12) + March (27) o Nitric Oxide, Antidepressants, and "Sexual Side Ef... o How Common Are "Sexual Side Effects"? o SSRIs, Weight Gain, and Diabetes o New Diabetes Risk Factor: Antidepressants o Heatmap Visualization of Antidepressants o Is Depression Really Biochemical? o New Patents Aim to Reduce Placebo Effect o Placebos Are Becoming More Effective o The "Good Data" Problem in Science o How Blind is Double-Blind? o The 10 Most-Prescribed Drugs in the U.S. o Life Expectancy for U.S. Women Heading Down? o Placebo Surgery o The Open-Source 3D-Printed Gun o A Call for Mandatory Publishing of Clinical Trials... o 77 Poor-Quality Antidepressant Studies o Biggest Risk Factor for Depression: Low Income o Abilify Tops the Drug Best-Seller List o Online Tests to See If You're Depressed o Wheat and Schizophrenia o Olive Oil, Fullerene, and Lifespan Extension o U.S. Life Expectancy Stalled o The Myth of Japanese Longevity o Body Mass Index vs. Longevity: Latest Findings o Life Expectancy and Mortality: Web Resources o How to Live 30% Longer (Maybe More) o Should We Prevent Disease, or Slow Down Aging? + February (28) + January (30) * 2012 (46) + December (18) + November (4) + October (1) + September (3) + August (1) + June (2) + May (4) + April (1) + March (2) + February (4) + January (6) * 2011 (15) + December (8) + November (1) + October (2) + September (1) + May (1) + April (2) * 2010 (84) + November (3) + October (2) + September (2) + August (10) + July (14) + June (2) + May (4) + April (3) + March (12) + February (19) + January (13) * 2009 (169) + December (7) + November (5) + October (1) + September (12) + August (12) + July (6) + June (13) + May (15) + April (29) + March (30) + February (21) + January (18) * 2008 (57) + December (6) + November (22) + October (16) + September (10) + August (2) + February (1) * 2007 (10) + December (3) + July (1) + March (3) + January (3) * 2006 (54) + December (1) + November (2) + October (2) + September (5) + August (3) + July (3) + June (5) + May (5) + April (8) + March (10) + February (8) + January (2) More View Kas Thomas's profile on LinkedIn Stay in Touch! Subscribe to our Almost Normal mailing list Follow my girl Sally's progress as she writes and publishes her schizoaffective memoir, Almost Normal, an outgrowth of "How I Fell in Love with a Schizophrenic." Sign up and we'll send one or two updates a month ONLY, not a torrent of spam. Your e-mail address will NOT be shared. Easy unsubscribe. No hassles. No baloney. No kidding. * indicates required Email Address * ____________________ Email Format * (*) html * ( ) text * ( ) mobile Subscribe Powered by Blogger. § #home PLOS ONE: New Articles Advertisement IFRAME: a3ac9da4 * plos.org * create account * sign in PLOS ONE * Subject Areas * For Authors Submit your Manuscript + Fair, rigorous peer review + Broad scope and wide reach get started + Why Publish with PLOS ONE + Publication Criteria + Editorial Policies + Preparing A Manuscript + Figure and Table Guidelines + Supporting Information Guidelines + Submitting a Manuscript * About Us + Journal Information + Editorial Board + Reviewer Guidelines + Article-Level Metrics + Open-Access License + Media Downloads + Guidelines for Comments and Corrections + Help Using this Site + Contact Us Search Search ____________________ SEARCH advanced search Open Access Peer-Reviewed Research Article Are All Placebo Effects Equal? Placebo Pills, Sham Acupuncture, Cue Conditioning and Their Association * Jian Kong mail, * E-mail: kongj@nmr.mgh.harvard.edu Affiliation: Psychiatry Department, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Rosa Spaeth, Affiliation: Psychiatry Department, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Amanda Cook, Affiliation: Psychiatry Department, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Irving Kirsch, Affiliations: Program in Placebo Studies, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, School of Psychology, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom X * Brian Claggett, Affiliations: Division of Cardiovascular Medicine, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Biostatistics Department, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Mark Vangel, Affiliation: MGH/MIT/HMS Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Randy L. Gollub, Affiliation: Psychiatry Department, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Jordan W. Smoller, Affiliation: Psychiatry Department, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Ted J. Kaptchuk Affiliation: Program in Placebo Studies, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America X * Published: July 31, 2013 * DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067485 * Article * About the Authors * Metrics * Comments * Related Content * Reader Comments (1) * Figures Abstract Placebo treatments and healing rituals have been used to treat pain throughout history. The present within-subject crossover study examines the variability in individual responses to placebo treatment with verbal suggestion and visual cue conditioning by investigating whether responses to different types of placebo treatment, as well as conditioning responses, correlate with one another. Secondarily, this study also examines whether responses to sham acupuncture correlate with responses to genuine acupuncture. Healthy subjects were recruited to participate in two sequential experiments. Experiment one is a five-session crossover study. In each session, subjects received one of four treatments: placebo pills (described as Tylenol), sham acupuncture, genuine acupuncture, or no treatment rest control condition. Before and after each treatment, paired with a verbal suggestion of positive effect, each subject's pain threshold, pain tolerance, and pain ratings to calibrated heat pain were measured. At least 14 days after completing experiment one, all subjects were invited to participate in experiment two, during which their analgesic responses to conditioned visual cues were tested. Forty-eight healthy subjects completed experiment one, and 45 completed experiment two. The results showed significantly different effects of genuine acupuncture, placebo pill and rest control on pain threshold. There was no significant association between placebo pills, sham acupuncture and cue conditioning effects, indicating that individuals may respond to unique healing rituals in different ways. This outcome suggests that placebo response may be a complex behavioral phenomenon that has properties that comprise a state, rather than a trait characteristic. This could explain the difficulty of detecting a signature for “placebo responders.” However, a significant association was found between the genuine and sham acupuncture treatments, implying that the non-specific effects of acupuncture may contribute to the analgesic effect observed in genuine acupuncture analgesia. Citation: Kong J, Spaeth R, Cook A, Kirsch I, Claggett B, et al. (2013) Are All Placebo Effects Equal? Placebo Pills, Sham Acupuncture, Cue Conditioning and Their Association. PLoS ONE 8(7): e67485. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067485 Editor: Sam Eldabe, The James Cook University Hospital, United Kingdom Received: February 17, 2013; Accepted: May 18, 2013; Published: July 31, 2013 Copyright: © 2013 Kong et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Funding: This work was supported by KO1AT003883 (NCCAM), R21AT004497 (NCCAM), R03AT218317 (NIDA), and R01AT006364 (NCCAM) to Jian Kong; R01AT005280 (NCCAM) to Randy Gollub; K24AT004095 (NCCAM) to Ted Kaptchuk; K24MH094614 (NIMH) to Jordan W. Smoller, and P01 AT006663 (NCCAM) to Bruce Rosen. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. Introduction Placebo treatments and healing rituals have been used since the beginning of human history [1], [2]. The systematic study of placebo and ritual is still in its infancy [3], [4]. Whether all placebo treatments, or medical rituals, have equivalent effects remains unknown. This raises the question: Do patients who respond to one placebo intervention also tend to respond to other placebo interventions? In a previous clinical trial [5] of chronic pain patients, we found that sham acupuncture reduced pain significantly more over time than did placebo pills, while placebo pills offered more short-term benefits of improving pain-disturbed sleep over sham acupuncture. Thus it showed that not all placebo treatments are equal. However, this clinical trial involved multiple, concurrent experimental arms and was not designed to answer the question of whether individuals who tend to respond to sham acupuncture also tend to respond to placebo pills. In another study [6], Colloca and colleagues compared the placebo effects of verbal suggestion and conditioning to a control condition and found that verbal suggestion alone could not produce significant differences in subjective pain ratings. Conditioning, on the other hand, could significantly reduce subjective pain ratings. The between-subject design of this study prevents the authors from elucidating the association between verbal suggestion and conditioning effects. Elucidating the relationship between different placebo modalities paired with verbal suggestion (suggestion-evoked placebo effects) as well as understanding their association with conditioning-evoked placebo effects will enhance our understanding of the variability observed in the placebo response. We are particularly interested in whether placebo responses can be characterized by the involvement of relatively stable traits or states, depending on particular circumstances. Acupuncture has been used to relieve pain in East Asia for two thousand years [7]. Recent clinical trials investigating acupuncture's effects on chronic pain have shown contradictory results and often fail to show superiority over sham acupuncture [8]. This ambiguity may be the result of acupuncture's sizeable placebo effects as well as large inter-individual variability in response to acupuncture treatment [5], [8]–[11]. It is well known that some patients respond well to placebo and acupuncture treatments while others do not [5], [9]. Thus, as a secondary aim, this paper also addresses whether individuals who respond to sham acupuncture also respond to genuine acupuncture. In this study, all subjects participated in two experiments sequentially. The first experiment was a multi-session crossover study [12] designed to test the analgesic effects of placebo Tylenol (pill), sham acupuncture, and genuine acupuncture (electroacupuncture) as compared to a no treatment (rest control). Subjects also participated in experiment two, in which we investigated how visual cues could modulate pain perception. These two experiments allow us to investigate the relationship between the analgesia evoked by different treatments as well as the analgesic effect evoked by conditioning cues. Methods Experiment one Subjects. Seventy-one healthy, acupuncture-naïve subjects were recruited from the community, using flyers and postings, and enrolled in the study. The Institutional Review Board at Massachusetts General Hospital approved the study and all subjects gave written informed consent before commencing experiment one. All subjects were told that if they completed experiment one, they would also be invited to participate in experiment two. Subjects were debriefed at the end of experiment two and investigators explained the rationale for deception during the experiments. All subjects found study procedures acceptable and agreed to have their data used for analysis. Outcome measurements. The outcome measures for this study include heat pain threshold and tolerance and subjective pain ratings of calibrated heat pain. All pain assessments were appliedusing a TSA-2001 Thermal Sensory Analyzer with a 3×3 cm probe (Medoc Advanced Medical Systems, RimatYishai, Israel) running proprietary computerized visual analog scale software (COVAS). Subjects' thresholds and tolerances to pain were assessed on the dorsal portion of the hand using an ascending method of limits paradigm with a rate of rise of 0.5°C/sec from a baseline of 32°C. The 3 cm X 3 cm thermode was held lightly in place on the skin by a member of the study staff blinded to treatment mode.Subjects pressed a button in front of them to indicate when the heat “first becomes painful”to indicate pain threshold and when the heat “becomes too painful to tolerate” to indicate tolerance. Three trials of pain threshold and tolerance assessments were performed. The thermode was repositioned between each trial. Calibrated heat pain stimuli were applied on the volar side of the forearms. All stimuli were delivered in 10-secondsegments (including an approximate 2.5 second ramp up and down from baseline) with a minimum inter-stimulus interval of 24 seconds. After each stimulus, the subjects were asked to rate the pain using the Gracely Sensory Scale (0–20) [13], [14]. Experimental procedure. The first experiment was a crossover study that involved five study visits, including one training session and4experimental sessions (Figure 1). Each session was separated by at least 3 days to avoid sensitization to the repeated application of the noxious stimuli and to allow for full recovery of the subjects' skin. Similar methods have been used in our previous studies and no damage or lesions have been observed [12], [15]–[17]. thumbnail Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image () * TIFF original image () Figure 1. Experimental design. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067485.g001 The first session was used to introduce the study procedures, determine appropriate stimulus intensities for each subject, minimize anticipatory anxiety to pain and acupuncture, and to control for rating strategy and learning effects. At the beginning of the training session (Session 1),subjects were told that in the subsequent 4 sessions, they would receive each of the 4 experimental conditions in a randomized order, i.e. two different modes of acupuncture treatment(electroacupuncture and manual acupuncture), one painkiller (Tylenol), and one control condition. Subjects were also told that before and after each treatment condition and the control condition, trained study staff would test their pain sensations to investigate the analgesic effect of different treatments on experimental pain applied to the forearm. In reality, the manual acupuncture was sham acupuncture and the Tylenol painkiller was a placebo pill. In addition, subjects were explicitly informed that the control condition was a baseline control for the study and, thus, no treatment effects were expected. To maintain uniform expectancy across treatment sessions, subjects were also told that the effects of the two types of acupuncture (electroacupuncture and manual acupuncture) and Tylenol treatments could work via different mechanisms, and that the efficacy of one treatment would not influence the efficacy of the others. The subjects were then trained to assess their pain threshold and tolerance. The heat stimuli were applied to the back of their hand in three separate locations for both threshold and tolerance measures. After that, they were instructed how to use the Gracely Sensory and Affective Scales [13], to rate calibrated heat pain with methods used in our previous studies [12], [15]–[17]. In order to identify the individually calibrated heat pain stimuli, a training set of ascending heat pain stimuli was administered starting from 38°C and increasing by one degree for each stimulus. From the subject's ratings of this initial series of stimuli, two heat pain intensities were determined for each subject: one to elicit responses in the strong range (“High pain”; 14–17 on the Sensory Scale) and one to elicit responses in the mild to moderate range (“Low pain”; 8–11 on the Sensory Scale). Sequences of the subject-specific High and Low stimuli were then applied in random order to separate areas of skin on the right volar forearm to further test the reliability of these ratings. Each random sequence consisted of six stimuli:3 Low pain stimuli and 3 High pain stimuli. The high and low temperatures were adjusted as necessary to reliably elicit a High or Low rating from a subject. Following these adjustments, the temperatures would remain fixed for all subsequent sessions. Next, acupuncture was administered for 2 minutes to introduce subjects to the acupuncture experience. As several previous studies [18]–[20] have suggested that optimism is associated with placebo effects, all subjects were asked to complete the Life Orientation Test (LOT) to assess individual differences in generalized optimism versus pessimism. Study sessions 2–5 were experimental test sessions. In each experimental session, a predetermined set of experimental heat pain tests was administered before and after treatment/conditioning in the following order: 1) pain threshold (3 times) on the dorsal portion of the right hand; 2) a pseudorandom noxious thermal stimuli sequence consisting of the 3 High and 3 Low pain stimuli on the volar side of right forearm; and 3) heat pain tolerance on the dorsal portion of the right hand. The dorsal portion of the hand was chosen due to its proximity to the acupuncture point used in this study. Heat pain was applied to separate areas of skin on the volar forearm in order to avoid the potential influence of one sequence of calibrated pain on each subsequent sequence. This predetermined set of tests was repeated during each experimental session. The only difference among the sessions was the treatment condition: electroacupuncture, sham acupuncture, placebo Tylenol, or rest control condition. The order in which subjects received the 4 experimental conditions was randomized prior to study proceedings using a computerized random number generator. Sequentially numbered method was applied for allocation concealment. To ensure that all subjects could consistently rate the discrete levels of heat pain, only subjects who demonstrated the ability to distinguish different pain intensities during Session 2 (i.e., on average, rating the High pain stimuli higher than the Low pain stimuli after application of the random noxious stimuli sequence) were selected to proceed in the study. Studies have shown that expectation can significantly influence an individual's perception of pain [15]–[17],[21]–[28]. Thus, all subjects were asked to complete the Expectations for Relief Scale (ERS) to indicate how much pain relief they expected from each particular treatment after receiving the treatment but prior to the post-treatment pain testing. The ERS is a ten-point scale (0–10) where 0 indicates a very negative expectation of “does not work at all” and 10 indicates a very positive expectation of “complete pain relief.”For the three treatment conditions, the subjects were told prior to treatment that they were about to receive a treatment that the investigators believed would have an analgesic effect on their entire body, including the arm. This served as verbal suggestion for the subjects in the treatment groups. For the control condition (30-minute rest period), subjects were still required to use ERS to rate how they expected their pain sensitivity would change after a 30-minute break, although they received no treatment. Interventions. Placebo Tylenol Pill: Subjects were informed that the goal of the session was to test the analgesic effect of a non-opioid analgesia pill, acetaminophen (Tylenol by brand), on experimental pain. They were also told that the pill would begin to take effect approximately half an hour after administration and that previous research has suggested that Tylenol can produce a general analgesic effect on the whole body, including the experimental pain applied to the forearms. After orally ingesting the pill (in reality an inert placebo pill), subjects waited about half an hour prior to the beginning of the post-treatment pain assessment. Sham (Placebo) Acupuncture: Sham acupuncture was performed using the validated Streitberger sham acupuncture device [12], . A small plastic ring was covered by an opaque, thin covering and then placed over2non-acupoints after the acupuncturist disinfected the area with isopropyl alcohol. A placebo needle, which was visually indistinguishable from genuine acupuncture needles, was inserted into the center of the ring and held in place by the tape. Because the Streitberger placebo needle has a blunt tip and a retractable shaft, the needle did not actually penetrate the skin; however the subjects felt a sensation similar to that of a pinprick or a scratch. After insertion, the needles were kept in place for 25 minutes. Two sham acupoints (sham large Intestine 4 and 3 (LI 4 and LI 3) were chosen [32].Sham acupoint LI 4 is located on the dorsum of the right hand, between the 1st and 2nd metacarpal bones, approximately in the middle of the 1st metacarpal bone on the ulnar side. Sham acupoint LI 3 is about one half cm above the metacarpal bones. Both needles were rotated until the subject reported some level of sensation. Then, all needles were left alone for the remainder of the procedure (about 23 minutes) without further manipulation. The subject was told that these proceedings constituted the manual acupuncture treatment. Genuine Acupuncture: The acupoints LI 4 and LI 3 on the right hand were used for the genuine acupuncture treatment. These points have well-documented analgesic effects in laboratory experiments [12], [33], [34]. After the acupuncturist located the acupoint and disinfected it with isopropyl alcohol, a small plastic ring was placed over the acupoint and secured with a thin strip of sterile plastic tape. This covering was used to maintain subject blinding to genuine and sham acupuncture conditions. A small alligator-type conducting clamp was then attached to each needle to create a circuit using an electroacupuncture device (OMS Medical Supplies IC-1107). A current was passed through the electrode at a continuous frequency of 2 Hz. The intensity of the stimulation was gradually increased to the highest level subjects could tolerate without the sensation of sharp pain. The electroacupuncture was applied for approximately 23 minutes. Immediately following each acupuncture treatment (genuine and sham acupuncture treatment), subjects quantified the sensations they felt around the stimulated acupoint using the MGH Acupuncture Sensation Scale (MASS) [12], [35]. No treatment rest control: The subjects in this group were told that the no-treatment condition was used as a within-subject control for the treatment conditions. Following the initial pain assessments, subjects were told to simply sit and relax for 30 minutes and wait for the post-treatment pain assessment to begin. Again, the subjects were asked to rate how much they thought the half hour rest period would change their sensitivity to pain during the second set of pain assessments. Experiment two All subjects who completed experiment one were invited to return to our lab at least 2 weeks after completion of experiment one to participate a one-session fMRI study to investigate the conditioning effects of visual cues. Please see original publication for more details on experimental procedures and fMRI results [36]. The present manuscript only focuses on the analysis of the association between the placebo effects evoked by suggestion in experiment one and the visual cue conditioning effects in experiment two. This analysis has not been included in previous reports. In brief, at the beginning of the experiment, subjects were told that the aim of experiment two was to investigate the brain's response to different levels of thermal pain. Subjects were then familiarized with the visual presentation paradigm, including a pre-stimulus cue, a pain stimulus symbol, and a post-stimulus rating scale. In addition, subjects were told that the pre-stimulus cue (text saying either “HIGH” or “LOW”) would indicate the level of the subsequent pain stimulus. Based on experiment one, temperatures that elicited subjective intensity ratings in the strong range (“High pain”; 14–17 on the Sensory Scale) and one to elicit responses in the mild to moderate range (“Low pain”;8–11 on the Sensory Scale)were selected for each subject and used in experiment 2 (MRI study). Immediately prior to the fMRI scan, a brief pain sensitivity test was performed to further confirm that the subjective ratings in response to the high and low temperature stimuli elicited were within the targeted range for the study and necessary adjustments were made. During fMRI scanning, 3 different series of pseudo-randomized pain sequences were applied to the distal portion of the right forearm above the wrist. Subjects were instructed to focus on a small black fixation cross in the center of the screen in front of them. The first scan was a contextual conditioning/learning scan where subjects were presented with a pre-stimulus cue, indicating (without deception) whether they would be administered a LOW or HIGH pain stimulus. The duration of the pain stimulus was 12 seconds and the intensity of the stimulus for this first sequence always corresponded to the pre-stimulus cue. After each pain stimulus, the Sensory Box Scale was displayed on the screen for 8 seconds, and subjects rated the intensity of their subjective pain by moving a cursor along the scale. In total, this learning sequence included 4 LOW and 4 HIGH pain stimuli. The initial conditioning scan was followed by 2 test pain sequences in which the LOW cue (LC) was sometimes followed by the HIGH pain stimulus (HP) (the LC condition), representing a condition where subjects were expected to report less pain in response to a suggested low stimulus, and sometimes followed by the LOW pain stimulus. Both test scans included nine stimuli, where 3 of the stimuli were cued as HIGH pain and six were cued as LOW pain. Following all HIGH cues (HC), a high pain stimulus was delivered (the HC condition). However, following 3 of the six LOW cues, a HIGH pain stimulus was delivered (the LC condition) instead of a LOW pain stimulus. All other timing aspects of the 2 test scans were identical to the first contextual learning/conditioning scan. The subjective pain ratings evoked by the different cues (LC or HC with identical HIGH heat pain stimuli) in runs 2 and 3 were used to calculate the conditioned cue effect. The cue effect was used to investigate the association between the analgesic effect of treatment observed in experiment one and the analgesic effect of visual cue conditioning. Analysis Experiment one The primary outcomes for experiment one were post-treatment measures of pain threshold, pain tolerance, and pain ratings (for high pain and low pain). Because each subject was evaluated under all 4 of the experimental conditions, the data were analyzed using repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with the corresponding baseline measures as the covariate and treatment as the factor of interest. A separate ANCOVA model was fit for each of the 3outcomes. In these ANCOVA models, session order and subject ID were also included as factors, eliminating the need to further control for subject-level covariates such as age and gender. For each subject outcome, the distribution of residuals from the ANCOVA model was visually inspected. In the event that noticeable non-normality was detected, a robust analysis rank ANCOVA was performed, replacing both outcomes and baseline scores with their respective ranked values [37]. If both normal-theory and robust analyses produced similar results, we concluded that the results were not likely to be sensitive to the normality assumption. For outcomes in which a significant difference among treatments was detected, regression analyses were used to investigate whether subject-level outcomes (including subject response to other treatments) were useful predictors of response to genuine acupuncture. Robust regression using M-estimation was employed to minimize the effect of outliers [38].All analyses were conducted using Stata (version 11). P-values of ≤0.05 were considered to be statistically significant. Experiment two The primary outcome for experiment two was the subjective pain ratings evoked by the different visual cues (Low Cue or High Cue with identical HIGH heat pain stimuli) in the 2 test pain sequences. We explored the association between the analgesic effect evoked by different treatments in experiment one (genuine acupuncture, sham acupuncture and placebo pills compared with rest condition, separately) and visual cue effects in experiment two by applying non-parametric Spearman correlations separately. For this analysis, we used the primary outcomes of experiment one (changes in pain threshold, pain ratings of calibrated pain stimuli and pain tolerance), and the primary outcome in experiment two (conditioning cue effects as indicated by subjective pain rating changes to identical pain stimuli). Results Experiment one Of the 71 healthy subjects who participated the study, 48 subjects (19 males, 34 white, 4 black, 6 Asian, 3 more than one race and 1 unknown) ages 21–37 (mean: 26.23, SE: 0.48) completed experiment one with data for analysis. One subject who completed experiment one was not included in data analysis due to missing data. Twenty-two subjects did not complete the study. The reasons for dropped subjects included scheduling difficulties (11), unstable pain ratings (6), inability to tolerate testing conditions (acupuncture or heat) (3), inability to understand nuances of pain rating scales (1), and voluntary withdrawal (1). The average intervals between treatments (baseline and each of the four conditions) were 8 days, 7.7 days, 7.5 days and 8.2 days. A summary of pain threshold and tolerance values and pain ratings of calibrated heat pain stimuli are shown in Table 1 and Figure 2. The ANCOVA (Table 2) showed that pain threshold post-treatment scores were significantly dependent on the mode of treatment (F = 3.57; df = 3, 137; p = 0.016). The distribution of the post-treatment scores, as well as the residual values from the ANCOVA model, was found to include outliers, which could potentially impact the results of the ANCOVA model. However, the sensitivity analysis conducted using rank data found a similar result (F = 3.63; df = 3, 137; p = 0.015). Post hoc analysis among the 4 experimental conditions showed that both genuine acupuncture and placebo pills produced significant post-treatment pain threshold increases (+0.79, 95% CI:[+0.25, +1.33], p = 0.004; and +0.74,95%CI: [+0.19, +0.1.29], p = 0.008 respectively) relative to rest control. No other significant differences between treatments were detected with respect to pain threshold (Table 3). thumbnail Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image () * TIFF original image () Figure 2. Summary of pain measurement difference (post- minus pre-treatment, mean ± SE) on pain threshold, pain tolerance, and pain rating across different groups. EA, electroacupuncture group; PA, placebo acupuncture group; PT, placebo Tylenol group; RS, resting control group. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067485.g002 thumbnail Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image () * TIFF original image () Table 1. Pre- and post-treatment pain threshold (centigrade), tolerance (centigrade), and pain ratings (intensity rating of high and low pain stimuli using the 0–20 GracelyScale) across different conditions (mean±SE). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067485.t001 thumbnail Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image () * TIFF original image () Table 2. Treatment effects across different measurements of pain sensation. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067485.t002 thumbnail Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image () * TIFF original image () Table 3. Post-hoc analysis of pain threshold: effect of treatment relative to rest control. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067485.t003 For pain ratings of calibrated heat pain stimulation and pain tolerance, no significant treatment effects were detected (Table 2). While the null hypothesis of global F-test for the effect of treatment on the pain tolerance outcome could not be rejected, it should be noted that an exploratory post-hoc analysis using a test for detecting trends across the treatment groups did reveal a significant progression in the strength of treatment effects when the treatments were ordered as rest control condition> Google Custom Search * Now Available on the Web The September/October 2013 Skeptical Inquirer * Archive Past Skeptical Inquirer articles now available online * Skeptical Briefs Selected articles from CSI's quarterly newsletter * Special Articles CSI's web-only exclusives * Guide for Authors A guide to submitting content for Skeptical Inquirer Tweets by @SkeptInquiry News & Announcements Skeptical Inquirer Is Now Available Digitally July 3, 2013 Skeptical Inquirer is now available digitally on Apple Newsstand and on all other major platforms through the Pocketmags app. Skeptic Authors Steven Salzberg and Joe Nickell to Receive Balles Prize in Critical Thinking June 14, 2013 Forbes columnist Steven Salzberg and author-investigator Joe Nickell will each be awarded the 2012 Robert P. Balles Prize in Critical Thinking, to be presented by the Committee for Skeptical Inquiry at the CFI Summit in October. Skeptical Inquirer Newsletter: May 2013 May 23, 2013 Playing dice with the Skeptic's Toolbox, Oliver Sacks and near-death experiences, and much more! The Mysterious Placebo * * Tweet * * Email Article John E. Dodes Volume 21.1, January / February 1997 One of the most significant but widely misunderstood phenomena is the placebo effect. Research shows that the placebo effect can be greater and is far more ubiquitous than commonly thought. If it was so, it might be; and if it were so, it would be; but as it isn't, it ain't. -Tweedledee, in Lewis Carroll's Through the Looking Glass One of the questions that skeptics are asked most persistently is to explain how acupuncture, homeopathy, faith healing, Qigong, and other treatments work. Skeptics often use the placebo effect-a response to the act of being treated, not to the treatment itself-as an answer, but usually to no avail. I believe that's because most people, both logical and fuzzy thinkers, don't truly understand what the placebo effect is. Spontaneous remission and the placebo effect, which are known as nonspecific effects, are significant phenomena that have great impact on consumers and health-care professionals. Recovery from illness, whether it follows self-medication, legitimate treatment, or avant-garde therapies, may lead one to conclude that the treatment received was the cause of the return to good health. A common saying is that if you treat a cold it will last a week, but if you leave it alone it will be gone in seven days. Even serious diseases have periods of exacerbation and remission; arthritis and multiple sclerosis are prime examples. There are even cases of cancers inexplicably disappearing. The major logical error in plotting disease progress is: post hoc, ergo propter hoc ("after it, therefore, because of it"). This common fallacy credits improvement to a specific treatment just because the improvement followed the treatment. H. K. Beecher's seminal paper "The Powerful Placebo" (Beecher 1955) is among the most frequently cited and was undoubtedly responsible for the double-blind study design having been adopted as the universal standard. Beecher reported on twenty-six studies and arrived at an average placebo response rate of 32.5 percent. From this figure comes the often cited statement that a fixed fraction (one-third) of the population responds to placebos. But this is a myth. A recent paper (Roberts et al. 1993) concluded that "under conditions of heightened expectations, the power of nonspecific effects (placebos) far exceeds that commonly reported in the literature." The paper, "The Power of Nonspecific Effects in Healing," is fascinating. The authors analyzed data for treatments that met the following criteria: * strong positive reports by at least two groups of investigators; * at least one well-controlled negative report; * the treatment had been abandoned as ineffective; and * data from a major portion of the positive studies were presented in a manner that permitted reliable classification into categories of excellent, good, or poor outcomes. The treatments that were studied included: glomectomy (the surgical removal of a small, normal mass of tissue called the carotid body that is found on the carotid artery) for the treatment of asthma; levamisole (an immunomodulatory drug) for the treatment of Herpes Simplex Virus (HSV); organic solvents (ethyl ether and chloroform), also for HSV; and gastric freezing for duodenal ulcers. In all the cases the doctors and the patients expected the treatments to work. The results for all the positive studies combined showed an excellent or good outcome in 69.8 percent of the almost seven thousand cases studied. These results led to the conclusion that a treatment outcome "is always due to some interactive combination of specific and nonspecific effects." Research also illustrates how difficult it is to separate valid treatments from apparently valid ones. In other words, we're never without some level of nonspecific effects. A number of other myths are associated with placebos. Try to answer the following questions: 1. Does a positive response to a placebo mean the patient's problem is imaginary? 2. Does a patient have to believe in the therapy for a placebo effect to occur? 3. Are placebos harmless? The answer to all three questions is no. Placebo responses can occur in patients with real disorders; the subjective symptoms can resolve while the objective ones remain. Belief in the treatment only appears to explain a portion of the placebo effect (Jarvis 1990). It appears that belief, operant conditioning, and suggestibility all play important roles. In an interesting experiment, a man experienced pain and exhibited marked depression of a specific part of his heartbeat while being monitored by an electrocardiogram (ECG) machine during a treadmill diagnostic test. This occurred at a treadmill setting of 44. For a second test, when the treadmill number was miscounted so that the patient exercised less, he exhibited the same pain and ECG depression as at the setting of 44 (Jarvis 1990). This dramatically demonstrates the power of suggestion. Contrary to popular belief, placebos can be harmful. Placebo responses can "teach" chronic illness by confirming and/or reinforcing the delusion of imagined disease (Jarvis 1990). Patients can become dependent on nonscientific practitioners who employ placebo therapies. Such patients may be led to believe they're suffering from imagined "reactive" hypoglycemia, nonexistent allergies and yeast infections, dental filling amalgam "toxicity," or that they're under the power of Qi or extraterrestrials. And patients can be led to believe that diseases are only amenable to a specific type of treatment from a specific practitioner. On the other hand, the practitioner can also be blinded to the real disease because of being convinced that the patient's condition is only imagined. Jarvis (1990) reminds us that "for both patient and practitioner to be blind to the clinical realities is an unacceptable version of the 'double-blind.'" The use of placebos can undermine the doctor-patient relationship by requiring deception on the part of the caregiver. Consumer advocate Stephen Barrett has explicit reservations concerning overreliance on the placebo effect in clinical practice: "I am against people being misled. The quack who relies on a placebo effect is also pretending he knows what he is doing-that he can tell what is wrong with you and that he has effective treatment for just about everything . . . he is encouraging people to form lifelong habits of using things they don't need" (Barrett 1977). In addition, placebos "need not always be beneficial and may be frankly toxic: dermatitis medicamentosa and angioneurotic edema (allergic-type reactions) have resulted from placebo therapy. More subtle but equally important negative placebo effects must occur when the physician by virtue of a moment of inattention, a raised eyebrow, or a transient look of disgust, loses the trust of his patient" (Bourne 1991). Paracelsus (Swiss alchemist and physician 1493-1541) wrote: "You must know that the will is a powerful adjuvant of medicine." It is imperative that skeptics recognize the wisdom and warnings inherent in this statement. References * Barrett, S. 1977. Health frauds and quackery. FDA Consumer 11:12-17. * Beecher, H. K. 1955. The powerful placebo. JADA 159:1602-1606. * Bourne, H. 1991. Unrecognized therapeutic measures, including placebo. In Clinical Pharmacology, ed. by K. L. Melmon and H. F. Morrelli. New York: Macmillan. * Jarvis, W. T. 1990. Dubious dentistry: A dental continuing education course. Loma Linda University, Loma Linda, Calif. * Roberts, A. H., D. G. Kewman, L. Mercier, and M. Hovell. 1993. The power of nonspecific effects in healing: Implications for psychosocial and biological treatments. Clinical Psychology Review 13:375-391. John E. Dodes John E. Dodes is the president of the New York Chapter of the National Council Against Health Fraud, and the only dentist on the New York State Department of Health's Health Fraud Advisory Board. His new consumer book, The Whole Tooth, written with Dr. Marvin Schissel, is to be published by St. Martin's Press in 1997. CSI logo About * About CSI * Contact * Committee for the Scientific Investigation of Claims of the Paranormal (CSICOP) * CSI Board, Fellows, and Staff * Donate to CSI * Educating Youth * Policy on Hostile Conduct/Harassment at Conferences * Privacy Policy * The Pantheon of Skeptics Resources * CSI Store * Skeptical Organizations in the United States * International Network of Skeptical Organizations * Centers for Inquiry * Free `I Doubt It' Decal * CFI Forums Publications * Subscribe * Skeptical Inquirer + Archive + Latest Issue * Skeptical Briefs + Latest Issue * Special Articles * Guide for Authors News * Events * Press Coverage * Press Releases * Recent Announcements Content copyright CSI or the respective copyright holders. Do not redistribute without obtaining permission. Thanks to the ESO for the image of the Helix Nebula, also NASA, ESA and the Hubble Heritage Team for the image of NGC 3808B (ARP 87). § [spacer.gif] [spacer.gif] [spacer.gif] [npi_logo.gif] [spacer.gif] Placebo Research at the UCLA Neuropsychiatric Institute [spacer.gif] [spacer.gif] Placebo Research Home Q & A about Placebos (FAQ) Our Research Team Press Releases & News Scientific Publications [spacer.gif] [spacer.gif] One of the most puzzling facts of medicine is the placebo effect: namely, that a substantial proportion of patients report feeling better after receiving a "sugar pill," or some other treatment with no known benefit for their illness. Between 30 - 60% of patients with illnesses ranging from arthritis to depression report a substantial improvement in their symptoms after receiving a placebo. It is not clear that placebo can "cure" any illness, but the power of the placebo effect in improving symptoms and reducing suffering is impressive. Our research program is designed to help us understand how placebo effects occur and how to distinguish between people who are exhibiting a placebo response and those responding to the specific treatment (e.g. medication). Our group is the first to report that when treatment with placebo reduces symptoms, it also changes the function of the brain. We examined research subjects with major depression, some of whom respond favorably to placebo treatment as part of research studies. When the subjects improved during placebo treatment, their brain function changed; the changes seen during placebo treatment were different from those seen during medication treatment. You can read the press release here and the full article was published online in January 2002 in the American Journal of Psychiatry. This work was replicated and extended in our December 2004 publication in Psychopharmacology. More recently, we have reported that brain function changes during a brief placebo treatment period can identify those who will later develop side effects when taking a real antidepressant medication. The press release and article from the April 2005 issue of Neuropsychopharmacology are both online. [spacer.gif] [ Neuropsychiatric Institute Home ] [ QEEG Lab Home ] § #Respectful Insolence » Feed Respectful Insolence » Comments Feed Respectful Insolence » The “myth” of placebo effects Comments Feed R.I.P., Seán Ó’Laighin Autism prevalence is reported to be 1 in 50, and the antivaccine movement goes wild…again Advertisement Science Blogs Go to [Select Blog..._____________________________________] Search National Geographic Search nationalgeographic.com ____________________ (BUTTON) Submit * Last 24 Hrs * Life Science * Physical Science * Environment * Humanities * Education * Politics * Medicine * Brain & Behavior * Technology * Information Science * Jobs Respectful Insolence The “myth” of placebo effects Posted by Orac on March 21, 2013 (90) More » Heidi Stevenson amuses me. The reasons are legion. Be it the time when Heidi lectured scientists on anecdotal evidence (which she values far more highly than scientists, of course, declaring it the “basis of all knowledge”); launched a vile and nonsensical attack on Stephen Barrett; argued against prior plausibility with using a straw man argument so massive that if it were set on fire (which she did) it could be seen from space; or made an even more idiotic argument to try to “prove” that wi-fi signals and EMF cause autism, Heidi never fails to deliver the stupid in mass quantities of black hole density. Of course, what does one expect? Stevenson is a homeopath, after all. Anyone who can believe the pseudoscience and wishful thinking that underlies homeopathy is already almost by definition lacking more than a little something in the logic department. Certainly she’s missing a lot in the science department as well. All of this is why she amuses me so much and why on occasion when she catches my attention I feel like laying down just a touch of that not-so-Respectful Insolence that you all know, love, and crave. True, sometimes I feel as though I’m slumming, but in actuality Stevenson’s very useful in that she is very good at demonstrating logical fallacies and misunderstandings of science that provide a number of what we in the science and skeptical blogging biz like to refer to as “teachable moments.” Yesterday, Stevenson provided yet another in her long line of teachable moments in the form of a post entitled, Busting the Placebo Myth: Placebos Don’t Cure. In it, she rails against us nasty, pointy-headed skeptics who point out that placebos only make people think they feel beter and don’t actually make people better. The hilarious thing is that she uses studies that I’ve blogged before in order to come to exactly the wrong conclusion. Stevenson’s great that way. Even better, since I’ve blogged these studies before, I can try to get a handle on my logorrhea in this post. Well, no I can’t. As Hans Solo said, “Hey, it’s me.” I can keep it short for a while, but sooner or later the logorrhea always returns. Whether it will return during this post or not, you’ll just have to keep reading to find out. We’ll have to see if it works out. Either way, it’ll be entertaining and educational. I promise. Clearly, Stevenson is really, really peeved at the criticism of her quackery that labels it as “placebo medicine.” Of course, as I’ve discussed time and time again the vast majority of so-called “complementary and alternative medicine” (CAM) or, as it’s called now, “integrative medicine,” is based on placebo medicine and the idea that wishing makes it so. I realize that Stevenson doesn’t like that, but that’s just tough. Reality is reality, quacks are quacks, and science is science, and never the three shall meet at the same time. In any case, Stevenson starts out: One of the most frequent epithets tossed at people who make claims of alternative medicine is that it’s just the placebo effect. But that is based on the myth that placebos can cure. The reality, as clearly documented by science, is that they don’t. So the skeptics base their favorite claim on a myth, not reality, and most assuredly not on science. Stevenson then goes on a brief rant about what she refers to as the “myth” of placebos, including that placebos don’t heal, that placebos are a “complete flop,” that no one is ever “cured” by a placebo. Of course, these are not myths. They are more or less true, except for the second one. Even skeptics don’t say that placebos area a “complete flop.” we’re all for studying placebo effects and determining ways to maximize it during treatment with actual…oh, you know…effective medicine. What we don’t approve of is relying on placebo effects alone, which is what the vast majority of CAM is. Not only is it unethical to lie to patients that way, but it doesn’t help the pathophysiology of the condition being treated. To most ethical, science-based physicians, that’s a double whammy against using placebos that can’t be overcome. The first funny thing is that, in order to make her point, Stevenson in essence buys into this point. Rather than trying to argue for the “pwoerful placebo,” some like to put it, or The Secret, as I like to put it, she actually does her best to rubbish placebo effects as insignificant as an introduction to arguing that any condistions “cured” by alternative medicine couldn’t possibly have been cured by placebo effects. This is a straw man even bigger than the aforementioned straw man whose demolition with napalm-grade flaming stupid could be seen from space. That straw man is that skeptics dismiss “healing” by alternative practitioenrs as being due to placebo effects. The second funny thing about this article is that Stevenson relies largely on Ted Kaptchuk, the Harvard high priest of acupuncture studies in quackademic medicine in the United States. Just type his name into the search box of this blog, and you’ll see that I’ve discussed him many times before in the context of how the results of his studies do not show what he concludes that they show or what they are claimed to show by advocates and the press. It thus amuses me greatly that both of the studies that Stevenson chooses to use to demonstrate her point are ones that I wrote. One I wrote about a couple of years ago—along with practically every skeptical blogging doc out there. Amazingly, Stevenson more or less correctly interprets the study: The study used FEV1, Forced Expiratory Volume in 1 Second, to measure effectiveness. The graph on the left clearly shows that the drug placebo and sham acupuncture (another kind of placebo) had no curative effect, as they didn’t cause any change in FEV1. However, in the graph on the right, you can see that the subjects felt that both the drug placebo and sham acupuncture were nearly as effective as the actual drug, albuterol. Two things are clearly demonstrated. One is that a patient’s sense of well-being can readily be skewed by belief. The other, though, is that placebos have absolutely no healing benefit. It kind of creeps me out that that’s more or less what I concluded in my analysis of this particular study. She also discussed the infamous “placebos without deception” study by Kaptchuk that was sold as evidence that you don’t have to lie to patients in order to invoke placebo effects but, when examined more closely, shoed nothing of the sort. For a moment, I was wondering if I was losing my sanity. What kept me from getting too worried was the tone of the article, very confrontational to science-based medicine and skeptics. Also, the sarcasm. Given how good I’ve become at not-so-Respectful Insolence, I’m very much in tune with other people’s sarcasm, and Stevenson was being quite sarcastic. I knew there had to be a reason and that there would be a payoff, and I wasn’t wrong: The claims by skeptics that the placebo effect can explain away any and all results of alternative medicine are pure bunk. They’re based on a false belief, the idea that the placebo effect is so powerful that it can cure. That is nothing but a myth. They can palliate—make people feel better—but never cure. The placebo effect can be powerful in terms of people’s sense of health and welfare. However, no one is ever healed by a placebo. Therefore, when someone is actually cured by an alternative treatment, the false bravado of the skeptics, who invariably come streaming along shouting about the placebo effect, usually full of condescension and insults, is nothing more than that—hot air based on a belief that is founded only in myth. The simple fact is that placebos cannot cure. So those claims of successful treatment for diseases that are not subjective—such as cancer, autism, mastitis in cattle, skin conditions, or any other—cannot be denied with that off-hand line, “But, my dear, it can easily be explained by the placebo effect.” No, it cannot. And there’s the flaming straw man. In fact, I challenge Heidi Stevenson to produce an example of a skeptic dismissing an “alternative” therapy story in which cancer, autism, mastitis in cattle (where did that one come from?), or skin conditions were “cured” by alternative medicine as being just due to placebo effects? Cancer is a very good example. Many are the times that I’ve spoken about alternative medicine “cancer cure” testimonials, going all the way back to the very beginning of this blog and continuing forward. What you’ll find are explanations of the potential for spontaneous remission, what the difference between adjuvant therapy and curative therapy is and how alternative cancer cure testimonials confuse them, and a variety of other explanations. One thing you will not find is me trying to dismiss these cancer cure testimonials as being due to placebo effects. Indeed, if I ever saw a skeptic trying to do that, that skeptic might well himself become the target of a little bit of not-so-Respectful Insolence. And he’d deserve it, too. In a way, maybe Stevenson can be forgiven for thinking that the only arguments skeptics have against alternative medicine is to label it all as “placebo.” She is, as has been noted before, a homeopath, and if there is a form of “medicine” that is nothing more than pure placebo, it’s homeopathy. However, that doesn’t mean that pointing to placebo effects is the only weapon in the skeptical arsenal against the unscientific and pseudoscientific claims of alternative medicine. it is but one of many, and it’s generally only invoked—and correctly so—when claims about the ability of this quackery or that to relieve pain, anxiety, or other subjective symptoms. No one—and I mean no one—tries to dismiss alternative cancer cure testimonials, for example, as mere placebo effect. Ditto the issue of autism, the “cure” of which is generally discussed in terms of development (autism is a condition of developmental delay, not stasis) and not placebo effects. Improvements in skin conditions are usually explained by how such conditions often wax and wane. In the end, I can’t help but wonder how Stevenson’s fellow quacks will react to her dismissing the ability of placebo effects to cure as a “myth.” Somehow I suspect they won’t be so happy. At least, Mike Adams won’t. Keywords: Heidi Stevenson, homeopathy, placebo, quackery (90) More » Comments 1. #1 Heliantus March 21, 2013 Busting the Placebo Myth: Placebos Don’t Cure Can we quote her next time someone proposes to harness the placebo effect? 2. #2 palindrom March 21, 2013 Crowd: We need a cure! We need a cure! Dr. Hibbert: Ho ho ho. Why, the only cure is bedrest. Anything I give you would be a placebo. Woman: [frantic] Where can we get these placebos? Man: (points at truck) Maybe, there’s some in this truck! (Crowd knock over truck and a box of killer bees from it break out and attack the crowd. A man, thinking the bee is a vaccine, takes it and swallows it) Man: I’m cured! I mean, ouch! — The Simpsons, “Marge in Chains” 3. #3 palindrom March 21, 2013 On that Simpsons quote — the crowd was upset because the dread Osaka Flu was going around. Ironically, the Osaka Flu arrived in Springfield from Japan in boxes of “Juice Looseners”, which had been pushed on TV in an infomercial by quack Doctor Nick Riviera and washed-up movie star Troy McClure. Homer and others had ordered them: (Homer: “Gotta get a juicer! Won’t get chest pains from answering the phone any more!”); the juicers were packed by hyperconscientious Japanese workers working sick, and the rest followed. And that wasn’t even the main storyline. 4. #4 Dangerous Bacon March 21, 2013 Orac can sneer at homeopaths, but I bet he’s never cured a case of bovine mastitis. 5. #5 Mephistopheles O'Brien March 21, 2013 Actually, Stevenson’s argument is pretty good: 1. Placebos don’t cure anything, they just make people feel better sometimes. 2. Alternative medicines cure things. ergo, Alternative medicines are not just placebos. Now, if she could only prove statement 2 were true, she’d have a case. 6. #6 Leigh Jackson March 21, 2013 So Stevenson get’s placebos but doesn’t get that CAM is a synonym for placebo. So funny one just has to cry. Compare and contrast her take with that of Jeremy Howick of the Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine (University of Oxford) in todays Times: “Placebos work… if doctors are happy prescribing them, we should take them.” Placebos don’t have to be deceptive to be clinically effective says Howick, quoting Kaptchuk’s “open” placebo study for irritable bowel syndrome. With Kaptchuk it seems like you pays your money and you makes your choice. 7. #7 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 @ Helianthus: Ha. Some woo-meisters speak as if ‘placebo’ incorporated a species of healing energy that is unleashed whenever woo transpires allowing the body to heal itself perhaps through the liberation of its own energy in resonant response. In other words, sympathetic magic. In other placebo-centric news: AoA has been in an uproar about a new study that reveals (gasp!) a 2% autism rate ( a phone survey/ see LBRB). Jake strikes back: ” And of course, Roy Richard Grinker is bringing dow the reputation of GW by using his academic stature to deny the autism epidemic once again”. I’m sure he actually believe that. 8. #8 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 I lost comments due to an error on my part: Helianthus: Some woo-meisters speak as if placebo were a means to liberate the healing energy that naturallyresides within the body. Probably through resonance. In other words, magic. In other news: AoA carries on about a new autism rate of 2 % based on a PHONE survey. Jake complains about Prof Grinker destroying GW’s reputation by ‘epidemic denialism’. 9. #9 sophia8 March 21, 2013 mastitis in cattle (where did that one come from?) Can Homeopathy Cure Mastitis in Cows? 10. #10 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 Stevenson writes at Green Med Info and Gaia Health**. Here’s an article from the latter ( 2010): ” Big Pharma Scores Big Win: Medicinal Herbs Will Disappear in EU” She cites Verkerk ( ANH). ** talk about swampish sinkholes of unreason. 11. #11 Dangerous Bacon March 21, 2013 Can you do a phone survey of cows? (lows softly) 12. #12 lilady March 21, 2013 “….mastitis in cattle (where did that one come from?)” You’ve gotta love YouTube for testimonials…. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=PmI_Mb_bRoo 13. #13 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 There is one thing about placebos: when you actively pursue a treatment, follow a diet, execise as well as PAY for these interactions, you are motivated to believe that they have an effect. If you invest time, thought, energy, effort and money in something, you want to believe in it. 14. #14 Todd W. http://www.harpocratesspeaks.com March 21, 2013 @Denice Walter AoA carries on about a new autism rate of 2 % based on a PHONE survey. Well, they can criticize the CDC for conducting a phone survey, because they’re the CDC and, therefore, wrong. But when AoA conducts a phone survey, even when the results don’t say what they want them to say, the survey is slamdunk evidence that vaccines cause autism. Then again, in the same breath they criticize the study for being a “mere” phone survey, they also go on and on about “ZOMG! 1 in 50 now!” So is the survey reliable or not? Then there’s also Teresa Conrick’s “research” about Alzheimer’s and Autism sharing a connection: other than both starting with “A”, seniors and kids both get the flu vaccine! The horror! Right? Then again, Ms. Conrick is also the one who blamed thimerosal for her daughter’s eyes (which were blue at birth) changing color to a brownish green, rather than understanding that babies are commonly born with blue irises that change color as they age. 15. #15 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 @ Todd W.: Ms Conrick has produced the most baroquely inelegant and convoluted hypotheses that I have ever seen. That must be some osrt of an accomplishment. 16. #16 Todd W. http://www.harpocratesspeaks.com March 21, 2013 @Denice Walter Her technique can be summed up thusly: “! Research! Dots! Connect them!” and then she drops the mic and walks off stage. And on-topic, when, oh when!, will people stop citing Kaptchuk? Especially that study purporting to look at “not misleading people about placebos” that actually did mislead them? 17. #17 Todd W. http://www.harpocratesspeaks.com March 21, 2013 Whoops. Forgot that pointy brackets are HTML code delimiters. That should have been “[Random quotes that sound scary]! Research! Dots! Connect them!” 18. #18 Bronze Dog March 21, 2013 The absolute most I think I’d ever say about cancer and placebos is that a placebo might make the patient feel better about the subjective symptoms. It wouldn’t do anything whatsoever about the cancer itself or any of the objectively measurable symptoms. If the cancer coincidentally went into remission, I wouldn’t give the placebo any credit for it, but alties probably would, since they so commonly rely on the post hoc fallacy. 19. #19 Krebiozen March 21, 2013 Stevenson’s article is one of the most jaw-droppingly dumb I have read in a long time. Homeopaths and acupuncturists make a living by persuading patients to mistake a placebo effect for a real effect of their treatment. A paper on placebo use came out today:Placebo Use in the United Kingdom: Results from a National Survey of Primary Care Practitioners Placebo use is common in primary care but questions remain about their benefits, harms, costs, and whether they can be delivered ethically. Further research is required to investigate ethically acceptable and cost-effective placebo interventions. 20. #20 Krebiozen March 21, 2013 The press release from Southampton University about that paper is also interesting: “This latest study with the University of Oxford demonstrates that doctors are generally using placebos in good faith to help patients,” says Professor George Lewith, co-lead author of the study from the University of Southampton. “Other previous published studies by Southampton have clearly shown placebos can help many people and can be effective for a long time after administration. The placebo effect works by releasing our body’s own natural painkillers into our nervous system. In my opinion the stigma attached to placebo use is irrational, and further investigation is needed to develop ethical, cost-effective placebos.” That’s not quite how I see placebos. Has it really been demonstrated that placebos work, “by releasing our body’s own natural painkillers into our nervous system”? I thought much of what is called “the placebo effect” isn’t really an effect at all, but an illusion collectively caused by regression toward the mean and various cognitive biases. If we are talking about subjective changes due to suggestion, perhaps we should call them that, and abandon the term “placebo effect” altogether as hopelessly misleading. 21. #21 Mike Olson Sherrard, Illinois March 21, 2013 Hi there! Lay person here. I have a liberal arts degree and worked as a lab tech in the Navy. I read popular science books, (some of which various science bloggers have written) and “Scientific American.” Just trying to give you an idea of my background. I read a book called something like, “Hacks, Quacks and Big Pharma flacks.” Which seemed to truly contradict the notion that placebos did nothing to help a person feeling better. Simply believing a med or procedure would work better or make a person feel better actually induced immunological responses…according to the book. Responses to name brand drugs were quicker than generics, as I recall. I don’t bring this up to contradict any argument against snake oil salesmen( or women), but rather to suggest that a premise of, “Placebos just make a person feel better it they don’t make them better,” is probably wrong. At least from what this ill informed, somewhat literate lay person has read. Can anyone clarify this for me? 22. #22 Narad March 21, 2013 mastitis in cattle (where did that one come from?) Taken apart by SkeptVet here. 23. #23 Alan Kellogg Soon to be repurposed City Library March 21, 2013 Drugs don’t heal people, drugs make it easier for people to heal themselves. 24. #24 Beamup March 21, 2013 I read a book called something like, “Hacks, Quacks and Big Pharma flacks.” Which seemed to truly contradict the notion that placebos did nothing to help a person feeling better. Simply believing a med or procedure would work better or make a person feel better actually induced immunological responses…according to the book. You can measure certain physiological responses, certainly. What nobody’s yet been able to do convincingly is show that they are MEANINGFUL rather than simply measurable. The classic example is that acupuncture produces changes in the brain visible in an fMRI. Well, sure. The brain registers that it’s feeling something. Does this mean that acupuncture actually does anything meaningful? No, in order to say that you’d have to measure an actual clinical outcome. In short, saying “placebo works, look, I can measure a change in this lab value” can be accurately translated as “look, placebo doesn’t really do anything, since the most effect I can find is in some random lab value rather than talking about a clinical result.” 25. #25 St Thomas March 21, 2013 https://www.youtube.com/watch?feature=player_embedded&v=ntWO7jnOcWE How placebo prescribing may work in practice. 26. #26 Leigh Jackson March 21, 2013 @ Krebiozen, 18,19 The oh-so-cautious official conclusion of the study regarding the scientific and ethical status of placebos belies the two co-leads loud and laudatory public endorsement of GPs use of placebos. The only unequivocal thing the study recommends is further research on placebos. Hardly surprising given that Lewith heads one of the two co-funders of the study. “The Southampton Complementary Medical Research Trust was specifically established to raise money and aid the research development at the Complementary Medicine Research Unit at the University of Southampton.” http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1193549/ And turkeys do not vote for Christmas: http://www.drrogersprize.org/files/bio-lewith.php 27. #27 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 I would so totally NOT go with concept of the placebo effect being an expression of endogenous opiates. 28. #28 S March 21, 2013 Doctors that prescribe placebos to their patients should be paid with placebo cash. Turnabout is fair play, right? 29. #29 Krebiozen March 21, 2013 Mike Olson, Simply believing a med or procedure would work better or make a person feel better actually induced immunological responses…according to the book. I think this is a common misconception. Most clinical trials compare an active treatment to a placebo, and it is easy to assume that any measured changes in patients in the placebo arm of the study are caused by the placebo. If you look at clinical trials that include a ‘no treatment’ arm, you will usually find that patients who have no treatment do just as well as those given a placebo. My favorite example is this study which is often cited as evidence that sham knee surgery (making an incision and then simply sewing it back up) is as effective as lavage (washing out the joint) and debridement (basically scraping out the joint), thus demonstrating the awesome power of the placebo. However: The authors found that all three treatment groups fared equally: each reported subjective symptomatic relief, but no objective improvement in function was noted in any of the groups. In other words, none of these interventions had any objective effect on knee function, only on subjective assessment of pain, and we don’t even know if patients given no treatment at all might have reported a similar reduction in pain over time, since the study lacked a no treatment group. The placebo wasn’t awesome at all, it was as useless as the surgical interventions. Similarly, you can find lots of studies of alternative therapies that are essentially placebos, for example therapeutic touch, that have no consistently measurable effect on an objective measure, for example, wound healing, as in this study. If you can find a study that shows a placebo has a consistent and significant objectively measurable effect as compared to no treatment I would be very interested. 30. #30 Krebiozen March 21, 2013 Leigh Jackson, Thanks for the information on the authors of that placebo study, which confirms my suspicions. 31. #31 Denice Walter March 21, 2013 Interestingly, one of the winners of the prize in 2007 was Abram Hoffer of Orthomolecular fame. If his ideas win a prize, I’d hate to read what the runners up and losers promulgate. 32. #32 Mike Olson March 21, 2013 Krebiozen Thank you. What you’ve reported sounds very similar to what was written in the book. I’d thought it had been mentioned in that same book, however, that if persons had some sort of infection and were given antibiotic, their immune system would respond with a given set of antibodies. Over time, even if they were given a placebo that appeared similar to the antibiotic, their bodies would still respond with the same antibodies. It seemed like an odd effect…perhaps I read it wrong. The author did indicate that essentially all persons are susceptible to the placebo effect and it had nothing to do with being “weak minded” or some defect of character. Again your response seems to be in the same vein as what he had written. 33. #33 Will Bradbury March 21, 2013 https://sphotos-b.xx.fbcdn.net/hphotos-frc3/253777_623247231024744_ 1877166513_n.jpg 34. #34 Narad March 21, 2013 @Will: That tangentially reminds me that back when I was an undergrad, we were sneaking Zilog chips to Roald Sagdeev. 35. #35 lkr March 21, 2013 Homeopathic cures for mastitis in cattle PLUS Raw milk for ADD, allergies, colds, etc. etc. in unvaccinated kids — what could go wrong? 36. #36 Darwy Røde grøde med fløde March 22, 2013 A telephone survey for cows? Let me fire up the grill… I think we’re having steaks tonight… 37. #37 herr doktor bimler March 22, 2013 Homeopathic cures for mastitis in cattle I am waiting for the acupuncture version. Tell you want, *you* stand behind Buttercup and stick the needles in, *I’ll* watch from a safe distance. 38. #38 Martin March 22, 2013 HDB: That sounds likely to result in the joke whose punchline is “If you can persuade my wife I was going to milk that cow, I’ll buy a tractor from you.” 39. #39 Krebiozen March 22, 2013 Mike Olson, Over time, even if they were given a placebo that appeared similar to the antibiotic, their bodies would still respond with the same antibodies. I suspect you mean ‘antigen’, not ‘antibody’, and I think you are referring to learned histamine release. Our immune system does seem to be susceptible to Pavlovian conditioning to some extent – this study found you can condition a rat’s immune systems to respond to an audiovisual cue, for example. It’s interesting, but I don’t see how that means placebos ‘work’ in the sense of having an objective effect on the body. It sounds to me as if the author of that book is looking for explanations of how placebos ‘work’, when the best evidence suggests that they don’t. 40. #40 Krebiozen March 22, 2013 Maybe I should declare myself as a lapsed placebo-effect believer – a few years ago I was hopeful that the field of psychoneuroimmunology would revolutionize medicine, but I have been sorely disappointed. Another example I find interesting is in a TV show, ‘Miracles For Sale’ (blocked in the UK) in which Derren Brown’ teaches a man many of the tricks of faith healing. The section I am thinking of is where he goes out on the street and ‘heals’ people – you will find it starting at 56:38 – the very first person who is ‘healed’ reports a pain in his leg reduces from a level of 10 down to zero. That’s pure suggestion, but I would be willing to bet that there was no objective improvement in his leg. I think that’s the extent to which placebos ‘work’. As to whether this is clinically useful, I think that’s debatable. In the sense that a placebo may persuade a patient to go away and stop bothering a doctor, then perhaps they have a use. On the other hand prescribing antibiotics for viral infections as a sort of placebo has contributed to the spread of resistant bacteria, and feeling better when you’re not can be dangerous, in asthma for example. 41. #41 Krebiozen March 22, 2013 To confuse matters I wrote above in #39, “I suspect you mean ‘antigen’, not ‘antibody’”, when I meant, “I suspect you mean ‘antigen’, not ‘antibiotic’. Hopefully you got my drift. 42. #42 Dr. Nancy Malik Gurgaon, India March 22, 2013 Placebo is an inactive dummy/inert pill (substance such as lactose/saline which do not alter the disease condition) prescribed to the patient for enhancing he non-specific effects of the treatment and/or the doctor does not want to disturb the course of action of previously prescribed medicine till it completes its own run. Ref: http://drnancymalik.hpathy.com/2012/08/17/understanding-evidence/ 43. #43 Bronze Dog March 22, 2013 One additional aspect I think is worth mentioning when it comes to subjective improvement in pain and the like: Some people are naturally polite, encouraging, or adverse to confrontation. If they see a doctor working to help them, they might report improvement, even if there is none, to avoid impugning the doctor’s competence. That’s probably one of the more subtle components of the placebo effect: Some patients will lie, understate, or exaggerate to make the doctor feel better. Add in the popular altie methods of blaming the victim, and some will feel pressured to always report improvement and cover up symptoms in order to avoid being shamed by their quack, guru, and/or altie friends. 44. #44 Krebiozen March 22, 2013 I see we have been subjected to a no-doubt hit-and-run visit from Nancy Malik who has been described as a “homeopathy shill, fact-blind delusional crank and serial comment spammer” . I suppose that’s appropriate for a post about placebos. 45. #45 Beamup March 22, 2013 @ Nancy: That is not accurate. The concept of a placebo is very much broader. A nice chat with a “practitioner” who assures you they can fix anything that’s wrong, for example, would very much qualify by the standard definition. Of course, I can’t expect anyone who claims to practice “evidence-based homeopathy” to understand that. The only legitimate evidence-based view of homeopathy is that it is completely bogus and does nothing… because it is, in fact, a placebo. 46. #46 Krebiozen March 22, 2013 Bronze Dog, Some patients will lie, understate, or exaggerate to make the doctor feel better. I agree, and none of these are helpful to the patient in the slightest. They may even discourage her/him from giving the doctor important information about her/his condition. 47. #47 Todd W. http://www.harpocratesspeaks.com March 22, 2013 Guys, you’re being too hard on Nancy Malik. You see, she’s describing homeopathy: an inactive dummy/inert pill (e.g., lactose) that does not alter the disease condition. 48. #48 Tom Herling March 22, 2013 Audio engineers will often rely on placebo effect to placate some impossible client who, during a recording session wants more “purple”–our code for some inaudible but highly desired effect. So knowing that an audio console usually has several redundant controls, we show our client a knob that does nothing, and let them operate it so they have control over the amount of “purple.” Then we rehearse a couple of times (“Did you get it?” we ask, “Almost, let’s try it again,” says the client) until we get it “right” and can head home, but only after we’ve reached our desired amount of billable hours. 49. #49 Leigh Jackson March 22, 2013 Re: placebo, qua endogenous opiates cum psychoneuroimmunology cum clasical conditioning. Would be interested to hear views on the work of Fabrizzio Benedetti. Typically, for example: http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/366/1572/1790.long In similar vein, Zubieta/Stoler: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19338509?dopt=Abstract 50. #50 Krebiozen March 22, 2013 Leigh Jackson, I’ll read and respond to the material you linked to in more detail in a day or two when I have had time to ponder it properly, as it’s an area that still greatly interests me. Briefly, placebos do appear to have an effect on perceived pain, but I wonder if that is as significant as the authors you refer to think. If I stomp on someone’s foot it may increase their endorphin levels and/or distract from a pain elsewhere in their body, and this effect would also probably be blocked by an opiate antagonist like naloxone. I suspect the effect of placebos on subjectively assessed conditions such as pain may be similar to this. The important question is whether this is clinically useful, and I’m not sure it is. 51. #51 Politicalguineapig March 23, 2013 Here cookie 52. #52 Leigh Jackson March 23, 2013 Krebiozen, Thanks. We think alike. Howick and Lewith speak as if it’s all a done deal: placebos do produce clinical benefits and the mechanism is understood. They are calling on the GMC to allow free prescription of placebos on the basis that it already happens and that research like Benedetti’s validates the practice. I trust that the GMC has a fuller picture than I do on the overall state of science in these matters, but my own (non-physician, non-researcher) position is profoundly sceptical. 53. #53 Melissa G March 23, 2013 So, speaking of placebo effects, and it’s evil Mirror Universe twin, nocebo here’s a Slate article about wind farms and people who live near them freaking out. My question is, why don’t people ever ascribe HEALTH benefits to stuff like this??? Why isn’t it ever, “Infrasound cured my cancer!” or “Cell phones boost your brainpower!!!” or the like? Humans seem to be primed to accept the negative about stuff we don’t fully understand, rather than the positive. I would *think* it’s maybe a trait that helped us survive the dangerous predators and poisonous new foods or something… only people seem ready to accept ANY health claim for weird foods that taste disgusting (goji berries, anyone? :P ). What’s up with people, y’all? 54. #54 Krebiozen March 23, 2013 Melissa G, You reminded me of this story from South Africa in which a microwave tower was alleged to be causing continuing, “headaches, nausea, tinnitus, dry burning itchy skins, gastric imbalances and totally disrupted sleep patterns”, before it was revealed it had been turned off for the previous six weeks. That’s another fine example of a nocebo effect. I have been reading the studies Leigh referred to, and some others, but I’m feeling more confused, if anything. For example they found that only 30-40% of patients respond to placebos, and that some types of placebo-induced analgesia are blocked by naloxone (which is an opioid inverse agonist, not an antagonist as I stated above) while others are not. It seems weird to me that the human body can produce effective endogenous analgesics, but only does so when it believes it has been given an external treatment. Even if this is correct, a drug for pain (or nausea etc.) that is ineffective in up to 70% of patients doesn’t seem very useful to me. By way of taking a step back from this, I looked at the Wikipedia article on placebos for the first time in several years, and found it very useful. It discusses, for example, Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche’s Cochrane review (PMID: 20091554) of placebos that concludes: We did not find that placebo interventions have important clinical effects in general. However, in certain settings placebo interventions can influence patient-reported outcomes, especially pain and nausea, though it is difficult to distinguish patient-reported effects of placebo from biased reporting. The effect on pain varied, even among trials with low risk of bias, from negligible to clinically important. Variations in the effect of placebo were partly explained by variations in how trials were conducted and how patients were informed. Oddly, Wikipedia mentions criticisms of this review on the grounds that: Their meta-analysis covered studies into a highly mixed group of conditions. It has been reported that for measurements in peripheral organs the placebo effect seems to be more effective in achieving improvements in physical parameters (such as decreasing hypertension, improving FEV1 in asthma sufferers, or decreasing prostatic hyperplasia or anal fissure) than in improving biochemical parameters (such as cholesterol or cortisol) in various conditions such as venous leg ulcers, Crohn’s disease, urinary tract infection, and chronic heart failure. Placebos also do not work as strongly in clinical trials because the subjects do not know whether they might be getting a real treatment or a sham one. Where studies are made of placebos in which people think they are receiving actual treatment (rather than merely its possibility) the placebo effect has been observed. Other writers have argued that the placebo effect can be reliably demonstrated under appropriate conditions. Can placebos really achieve improvements such as “decreasing hypertension, improving FEV1 in asthma sufferers, or decreasing prostatic hyperplasia or anal fissure”? That question led me to this meta-analysis, which did a MEDLINE search for placebo-controlled studies on “peripheral disease processes”. However, I note that they state: Because the selected trials did not include untreated control groups, we restricted the dataset to trials on stable disease conditions, from which the effects of placebo treatments could be estimated by baseline changes within the placebo groups. No untreated control groups again, which leaves me feeling somewhat underwhelmed by the barely significant placebo effects on physical parameters they found in this and on reanalyzing Hróbjartsson and Gøtzsche’s data. I’m reminded of studies on homeopathy and acupuncture which show barely statistically significant effects that would be of little clinical usefulness even if they were reliably reproducible which they are not. I suspect many of the results Benedetti, Zubieta, Stoler et al have been pursuing are little more than artefacts in noise. 55. #55 Leigh Jackson March 24, 2013 We feel happy if good stuff happens to us: “here is something to help you feel better” – a placebo. The good stuff here is someone saying they can help us to feel better: lo and behold we do feel a little better! The placebo works? Nope. The good news worked – subjectively. Turn it all round: if we feel good then good stuff will happen to us. This is what needs to be true if a placebo is to cause objective clinical benefits. The good news that someone has something to help us makes us happy and then our feeling of happiness leads to our bodies repairing themselves. Obviously too good to be true except at the margins of bodily health. And then not at all easy to demonstrate. As for the idea of non-deceptive placebos, pull the other one. If you can see you are only getting a toothpick prick of your skin and not an acupuncture needle into your flesh you are not going to get the magical placebo benefit of acupuncture; now are you? For the magic trick to work you need deception. 56. #56 Dr. Nancy Malik Gurgaon, India March 24, 2013 @beamup Efficacy is the extent to which the medical treatment/intervention has the therapeutic effect under ideal test conditions 1. Specific (pharmacological/physiological) effects: patients getting better because of the therapeutic effects of the medical treatment/intervention. 2. Non-specific (psychological/placebo) effects:The body’s healing response is activated 57. #57 Chris March 24, 2013 And Nancy “robospammer” Malik, homeopathy is literally nothing and does not help with anything except to transfer cash from the patient’s wallet to your bank account. 58. #58 Krebiozen March 24, 2013 Nancy Malik, Non-specific (psychological/placebo) effects:The body’s healing response is activated Maybe I’m lucky, my body activates its healing response automatically, without any need for particular psychological conditions or a placebo, and rarely requires assistance from active outside agents. Why would the body require a placebo to persuade it to activate its healing response? 59. #59 Denice Walter March 24, 2013 @ Krebiozen: No no no! The placebo activates the inherent bioenergetic sytem (TM) which has been a-slumbering, its Xi ( qi, ki, mana, prana, life energy et al) obviously requires raw food, hand waving, energy medicine, alkaline water, chakra balancing, body work, acupuncture etc to inspire its get-go to get going. But for some reason, they never taught us about that at our universities. I wonder why? -btw- how are the foxes? 60. #60 Khani March 24, 2013 #53 I know at least one person who finds the sound of a wind tower to be very pleasant. Sometimes he drives out to one and sits under it for a while, listening–he says he finds it relaxing. Cheaper than therapy, right? Their sound remind me a bit of a washing machine, or maybe the ocean, personally. 61. #61 Shay The Illinois upland corn prairie March 24, 2013 We live six miles from a wind farm. It ruins the view but it hasn’t driven anybody here crazy. Yet. 62. #62 Krebiozen March 24, 2013 Denice, The placebo activates the inherent bioenergetic sytem [...] It seems like a serious design fault to me, having these marvelous inherent painkilling and healing abilities that only do their thing when we think something else is responsible. -btw- how are the foxes? There haven’t been many out and about recently; the mothers stay underground with their young at this time of year. We did see one with hair loss, probably sarcoptic mange today, the first we have seen in quite some time, which is worrying, though it may be an interloper from elsewhere, attracted by all the food available here. A few years ago we sent for some free fox mange medicine from an animal welfare charity, which turned out to be a homeopathic medicine i.e. a small bottle of distilled water. My wife used it, despite my skepticism, and I had to grudgingly admit it did seem to help though I suspect any real improvement was because of the bread and jam the medicine was administered on, which maybe provides some nutrients that the standard urban fox diet is lacking. Subsequent experience suggests that plenty of chicken scraps work just as well, which makes me wonder if it isn’t sarcoptic mange at all, but some sort of vitamin deficiency. Or perhaps placebos work on foxes, which neatly brings us back to the topic in hand. 63. #63 Khani March 24, 2013 #61 Ruins the view? I honestly find them very beautiful. I grew up on the prairie but I still find the flatness to be rather oppressive at times, and the wind towers add a nice vertical element, to me. Sometimes when I’m driving down the highway with wind towers on either side of my I do a little evil laughter and shout “THE POWER!!!! THE POOOOOWERRR!!!!!” … it’s a very long drive to visit my parents and with all the caffeine and sugar I consume to help stay awake, I get a little… funny. 64. #64 Shay March 24, 2013 Granted, there isn’t much of a view here unless you’re really into corn and soybeans. But what there is, was prettier before all them dang towers got put up. 65. #65 Denice Walter March 24, 2013 @ Krebiozen: I’m glad to hear that they’re still around…and multiplying. Nearby, our *suburban* foxes ( they all drive tiny black Jeeps and Range Rovers**) hang around a park on both banks of a small river that’s about 10 miles long- it has woods, sports fields and bike/ walking paths but also borders on many backyards- which means that they often “interact” with humans. Thus, the ex feeds them expensive cat food and my friend observes them when riding her bike or walking her son’s dog. The foxes I’ve seen look quite healthy. Probably all that expensive cat food and whatever they steal from the garbage of wasteful people whose backyards they inhabit. Foxes- living the high life. ** not really 66. #66 Khani March 24, 2013 #64 Corn and soybeans here, too, and where I grew up. I still prefer the towers–elegant white arms reaching to the sky. 67. #67 Narad March 24, 2013 I still prefer the towers–elegant white arms reaching to the sky. Yah, but do they explode like proper silos? 68. #68 Shay March 24, 2013 I can’t help it, Khani…they make me think of triffids. 69. #69 Khani March 25, 2013 #67 Well, sometimes they catch fire and their arms fall off. #68 Hee! Well, if one ever leans over and gobbles me or the car up, you may say “I told you so” with complete vindication! Perhaps they’re merely moving their armies into position prior to takeover… 70. #70 Narad March 25, 2013 Well, sometimes they catch fire and their arms fall off. I suppose that’s something. Sadly, a blog devoted to such events seems to have gone dormant. 71. #71 Krebiozen March 25, 2013 Denice, Foxes- living the high life. It’s funny to think of foxes having a sort of class system that mirrors that of the humans they coexist with. As I have no doubt mentioned before, the Thames flows west to east, so historically the further east you went the more polluted it was, resulting in the west to east spectrum of descending residential desirability we still see in London. I would like to imagine fat foxes in the wealthier west London suburbs lapping up dregs of Bollinger and nibbling at scraps of Beluga caviar, but in reality that class of Englishperson prefers to pursue foxes with horse and hounds, so I doubt they are particularly fox-tolerant. The foxes in my manor walk with a Cockney strut, drop their aitches and sprinkle their barks with glottal stops*. They are rougher and tougher than those effete west London foxes, having to live on the odd scraps of a kebab, burger or curry discarded by a drunk, apart from those lucky ones that frequent our back yard. * Or so I like to imagine. 72. #72 Krebiozen March 25, 2013 These videos, about the construction of the London Array of wind turbines, are well watching if you enjoy having your mind boggled by impressive feats of engineering. 73. #73 Dr. Nancy Malik Gurgaon, India March 25, 2013 Upto the end of year 2010, there have been 270 studies published in 106 medical journals including 11 meta-analysis, 8 systematic reviews including 1 cochrane review (out of approximately 20 systematic reviews published) and 93 DBRPCT (out of approximately 225 RCT published) in evidence of homeopathy. Ref: http://drnancymalik.wordpress.com/article/scientific-research-in-ho meopathy/ 74. #74 Grant http://sciblogs.co.nz/code-for-life/ March 25, 2013 Dr. Nancy Malik, “[…] in evidence of homeopathy.” … with the overall conclusion that it doesn’t work. I think something got left out there, eh? (I’ve little doubt her private list is biased to her interests, of course.) 75. #75 Grant March 25, 2013 (Should say doesn’t work beyond a placebo effect, but you all know that right?) 76. #76 JGC trying to cut to the chase March 25, 2013 Dr. Malik, let’s simplify the discussion as much as possible: what in your opinion is the single most compelling piece of evidence demonstrating that homeopathy is more effective at treating non-self-limiting illnesses than are placebo’s (i.e., that it actually works)? 77. #77 Dr. Nancy Malik Gurgaon, India March 25, 2013 @JGC There are different types of research. You can’t compare apples with oranges. There are meta-analysis and systematic reviews http://drnancymalik.wordpress.com/article/meta-analysis-and-systema tic-reviews/ There are research models http://drnancymalik.wordpress.com/article/research-models-in-homeop athy/ There are DBRPCTs and there are obeservational studies. Which one you like to discuss? 78. #78 Chris March 25, 2013 Awww, how cute. Robospammer Malik is referencing its own website. All in the effort to advertise ways to transfer cash from the wallets of the credulous to its own bank account. 79. #79 MI Dawn March 25, 2013 Dr Malik: We don’t want to compare apples and oranges. We are asking for studies published in reputable peer-reviewed journals, that either have been or can be reliably replicated by other researches. So unless you are claiming that your site articles are the same as those done in that literature I’m not going to follow your links. 80. #80 Science Mom http://justthevax.blogspot.com/ March 25, 2013 There are different types of research. You can’t compare apples with oranges. Surely you don’t think that non-answer is going to fly here do you? The question was very straightforward. Please do the courtesy of answering it. 81. #81 Science Mom http://justthevax.blogspot.com/ March 25, 2013 @ MI Dawn, just to let you know, “Dr.” Malik isn’t a doctor at all, just another homeopath crank. 82. #82 JGC March 25, 2013 There are different types of research. You can’t compare apples with oranges. You can’t compare fruit to fruit? Why not? Which one you like to discuss? As I stated previously I’d like to discuss what you believe to be the single most compelling study or piece of evdence demonstrating homeopathy is effective at treating illnesses that are not self-limiting. I’d hope this would take the form of a well designed and appropriately controlled blinded clinical study (but from your suggestion that homeopathy requires a ‘different type of research’ I doubt this would be the case.) 83. #83 Mark James Newcastle March 25, 2013 I’ve really enjoyed reading the debate between Krebiozen and Leigh Jackson. But do feel that there is a lot more to placebos than a subjective response of feeling better. Placebos cannot cure cancer, diabetes, cardiovascular disease etc. But they can improve pain perceptions. Specifically in sport and exercise. A reliable study by Beedie et al., demonstrated that although caffeine does improve performance, the expectations specifically influence how well you will do http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/18091009 McClung & Collins show that there are little differences between a supplement compared to a positive expectancy. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/17876973 There are many more in the sport and exercise field where a placebo can improve an athletes performance. If the person can run faster does that not mean the placebo has worked? 84. #84 MI Dawn March 25, 2013 @Science Mom: yeah, I know. But since she’s so fond of the title (and maybe can use it legally where she is?) I thought I’d go ahead and call her by it. If she practiced in the US, I wouldn’t. 85. #85 Grant March 25, 2013 Good reminder re her degree – I once took her to task with it on my blog, checking out what she had and whatnot. I can’t edit my earlier post, but strike the ‘Dr.’ portion. 86. #86 Mephistopheles O'Brien March 25, 2013 Mark James – perhaps you’ve seen the Disney movie “Dumbo”. Did the magic feather work? 87. #87 Krebiozen March 26, 2013 Mark James, If the person can run faster does that not mean the placebo has worked? It depends on what you mean by “worked”. I’m certainly not denying that placebos can have subjective psychological effects. Nor would I deny that those subjective psychological effects can affect a person’s neurochemistry, physiology or behavior, including sports performance, but so can a pep talk from a coach, being chased by a hungry predator and a million other things. I think the mechanism behind these effects is interesting, but I’m not convinced think they are clinically useful or ethical on their own, in the form of a sugar pill or a saline injection, for example. I also think it is worth maximizing these effects when a patient is given an active treatment as Orac suggested in the OP. Hypnotherapy is essentially a way of harnessing this element of the ragbag of things that are collectively referred to as “the placebo effect”, but without the need to deceive the patient. So are therapies like counseling, massage, yoga or taking a walk in the sunshine. BTW, the first study you linked to is a good example of the confusing results we see in this area; giving caffeine to cyclists improved performance, but more so when the cyclists were told they had not been given it? The effect size of being told they had been given caffeine when they had not was very small 0.7%, which isn’t very impressive. I wonder if this is just noise, or perhaps poor blinding. I don’t have access to the full text of the second study, so I can’t see how big the effect sizes were, but it would be interesting to know, though what the study really shows is that the ‘performance enhancing’ supplements they were testing had no effect above placebo. It would be interesting to compare the effects of a placebo with the effects of superstitious amulets like a pair of lucky socks, visualization, a pep talk etc. etc.. I strongly suspect the exact same phenomenon is involved. 88. #88 (Long) Weekend Reading | Science-Based Pharmacy March 29, 2013 [...] The “myth” of placebo effects [...] 89. #89 Antilusional April 12, 2013 “Dr” (cough) Nancy Maliquack isn’t being disclosing her cherry picking. 90. #90 KR TN May 14, 2013 Ughh, I loathe Heidi Stevenson. Her articles drive me bonkers. And whenever anyone brings up the obvious holes and omissions in her research about anything she blindly points her finger at people and accusing them of “fear mongering”. If you happen to best her or bring up an irrefutable point she will delete your comment and attack your rhetoric saying it doesn’t follow [her] rules of “debate”. People who are so one sided and biased about important issues should not be allowed to inform the public at large and present their ideas as the only true scientific “fact”. Impressionable people listen to this garbage, find a few emotional choice phrases to cling to and then use them in every subsequent argument as the be-all-end-all when the truth is: the surface has to be tapped. Search This Blog Search for: ____________________ Search Subscribe Subscribe via: * RSS2 * Atom * Subscribe via a feed reader Search for: ____________________ Search Recent Insolence returned * herr doktor bimler on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Vicki on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Nick K on A bit of antivaccine misinformation from the left and the right * Narad on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Johanna on Antivaccinationists abuse reporting algorithms to silence pro-vaccine skeptics on Facebook * Mrs Woo on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * novalox on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Militant Agnostic on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Narad on A bit of antivaccine misinformation from the left and the right * Spectator on Antivaccinationists abuse reporting algorithms to silence pro-vaccine skeptics on Facebook * Chris, on When false hope leads well-meaning people astray * Mephistopheles O'Brien on A bit of antivaccine misinformation from the left and the right * Orac on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Narad on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Militant Agnostic on When false hope leads well-meaning people astray * Greg on A bit of antivaccine misinformation from the left and the right * Narad on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him * Krubozumo Nyankoye on As 2013 draws to a close in the skeptical world… * Mrs Woo on Antivaccinationists abuse reporting algorithms to silence pro-vaccine skeptics on Facebook * Denice Walter on Chris beat cancer? He did indeed, but it wasn’t quackery that cured him Recent Insolence delivered * Antivaccinationists abuse reporting algorithms to silence pro-vaccine skeptics on Facebook This is not what I wanted to write about for my first post of 2014, but… * As 2013 draws to a close in the skeptical world… As I write this, 2013 is drawing to a close, with only a little more than… * Medicine if I were a time traveler like The Doctor… NOTE: Because I’ve been (kind of) relaxing over this holiday period, this is not an entirely… * Is “value-based’ health insurance really “value-based” or is it just an excuse to pay more? And now for something completely different… Unfortunately, it’s all too easy to find new woo-filled claims or… * In which Mike Adams gives Orac an unintended Christmas present It’s Christmas Eve, and the blogging is light. I was going to have some fun with… * Sarah Hershberger comes home to die It was a couple of weeks ago that I last provided an update on the case… * When false hope leads well-meaning people astray One of the frequent topics on this blog is, unsurprisingly, cancer quackery. Be it the Gerson… * Supplements: Flushing your money down the toilet in expensive urine I remember during medical school that more than one of my faculty used to have … * Why are antivaccinationists so at home with Libertarianism? Rats. Everyone’s blogging about all the studies showing (as if it needed to be shown yet again)… * Better late than never…again Whoa. How did I miss this? Maybe it was all the other stuff going on last week,… * Fear mongering over cell phones and cancer by Dr. Oz These days, Dr. Oz seems to stand for everything I oppose in medicine: Fear mongering, quackery,… * A bit of antivaccine misinformation from the left and the right Thanks, Daily Kos. Well, not really. You’ll see why in a minute, but first here’s the background.… Categories Announcements Anti-Semitism Antivaccine nonsense Autism Bioethics Biology Blogging Blog housekeeping Cancer Clinical trials Complementary and alternative medicine Computers EneMan Entertainment/culture Evolution Friday Woo History Hitler Zombie Holocaust Holocaust denial Homeopathy Humor Intelligent design/creationism Medicine Movies Music Naturopathy News of the Weird Paranormal Pareidolia Personal Physics Politics Popular culture Pseudoscience Quackery Religion Science Science fiction/fantasy Skepticism/critical thinking Sports Surgery Tales from the helicopter Television World War II Schedule of Insolence CAPTION: March 2013 M T W T F S S « Feb Apr » 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 Categories * Announcements (337) * Anti-Semitism (121) * Antivaccine nonsense (1032) * Autism (744) * Bioethics (167) * Biology (139) * Blog housekeeping (95) * Blogging (188) * Cancer (593) * Clinical trials (338) * Complementary and alternative medicine (1881) * Computers (74) * EneMan (37) * Entertainment/culture (367) * Evolution (193) * Friday Woo (152) * History (282) * Hitler Zombie (40) * Holocaust (159) * Holocaust denial (125) * Homeopathy (99) * Humor (510) * Intelligent design/creationism (170) * Medicine (3007) * Movies (89) * Music (125) * Naturopathy (60) * News of the Weird (109) * Paranormal (70) * Pareidolia (31) * Personal (79) * Physics (21) * Politics (611) * Popular culture (131) * Pseudoscience (332) * Quackery (1658) * Religion (319) * Science (584) * Science fiction/fantasy (91) * Skepticism/critical thinking (945) * Sports (40) * Surgery (201) * Tales from the helicopter (1) * Television (208) * World War II (110) Archives [Select Month________] Meta * Log in * Entries RSS * Comments RSS * WordPress.org __________________________________________________________________ © 2006-2014 ScienceBlogs LLC. ScienceBlogs is a registered trademark of ScienceBlogs LLC. All rights reserved. [p?c1=2&c2=3005368&cv=2.0&cj=1] § #alternate RSS search * Fast Company logo Fast Company * * Design * * Exist * * Create * * Labs * * * * Features * * Emails * * Issues * * Subscribe * * * * * * + Editions + + China * * + Find Us + + Facebook + Twitter ____________________ Search Close [3016012-poster-1280-pillss.jpg] [fastcoexist-dark-txt.svg] Co.Exist 2013-08-26 Co.Exist The Placebo Effect Is Real. Now Doctors Just Have To Work Out How To Use It A growing body of research shows that treatments ranging from sugar pills to the "ritual of medicine" really do improve patient health. Doctors could start to harness the placebo's power by making small changes. In the last decade, the study of placebo has moved from the backwaters of medical research to nearer the mainstream. There is now a Program in Placebo Studies at Harvard. NIH and nonprofits like the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation fund placebo work. Journals publish papers. What used to be dismissed is now taken increasingly seriously. It's fair to say this shift wouldn't have happened without Ted Kaptchuk, who heads Harvard's placebo program. Over 35 years, he's been investigating placebos, which are medical interventions without active drug ingredients, in various contexts and expanding the scope of the field--including, for a time, to his own acupuncture practice. "The placebo effect is brought about by much more than sugar pills and saline injections. It's about the whole "drama" or "theater" of medicine. " To Kaptchuk, the placebo effect is brought about by much more than sugar pills and saline injections. It's about the whole "drama" or "theater" of medicine--essentially the context of the encounter between patient and physician--as much as treatment itself. "The placebo effect is the effect of everything surrounding the fake pill, or the real pill," he says. "It's the compassion, trust, and care. It's the ritual and symbols. It's the doctor-patient interaction." Kaptchuk has published several papers that have gotten wider attention beyond the world of academic journals and conferences. For example, he got a big response for a study with irritable bowel syndrome patients, where he showed that placebo could be effective even when patients knew the treatment was fake. (The drug bottle had a big "placebo" label on it.) Though the study only included 80 volunteers, the results seemed to indicate something profound: When patients want to get better, and believe that doctors are there to help them, good things happen. Kaptchuk credits the growing respectability of studying placebos down to the wider availability of neuro-imaging techniques. By demonstrating physiological impact in the brain when a patient takes a placebo treatment, researchers can move beyond conjecture about how the effect works. "The discovery of neurobiology has made physicians in the medical community more comfortable that something is going on that they have to pay attention to," he says. "Before it was just the imagination. Now, the imagination has a real neurobiology." Researchers can begin to see inside the brain, helping to gauge what works and when. Neuro-imaging has allowed researchers to look at whether there's a genetic basis for why some people seem more susceptible to placebo treatments than others. The recent research findings do beg a serious question, though. Are we going to see actual treatments based on pills that are no more than sugar as alternatives to active drugs? How might medicine incorporate the work that Kaptchuk and other placebo researchers are bringing to the world? In some ways, it's already happening. A survey from the U.K., recently showed that many doctors are already prescribing placebos on a regular basis. Out of 783 doctors, 12% said they had used "pure" placebo (sugar pills, saline injections) while 97% had used "impure placebos" (prescribing drugs without scientific basis, like antibiotics for viruses). More than three-quarters said they did so at least once a week, and many didn't have an ethical problem with deceiving patients if treatments were effective. Kaptchuk is not in favor of deception. Nor does he think sugar pills cure cancer. He does believe placebo research can help with conditions where self-appraisal is important. "You can change a person's sense of the symptoms. The only thing we know for sure is that the ritual of medicine changes subjective outcomes and that there's a biological substrate to that," he says. "The placebo is going to be for things like headache and back pain, muscular skeletal pain, and digestive and urinary problems." Kaptchuk says small changes in how drugs are delivered can improve a patient's well-being. For example, if you inject someone with morphine, and tell them what they're getting, it has double the effect of putting an IV in their arm and telling them nothing. That puts more responsibility on the medical community to provide care, rather than just relying on drugs to do the work. "Placebo is about how we improve care independent of medication. It really puts the onus on the physician, because it says, 'if you're not doing this, it's actually bad care.' In the pharmaco-centric world that medicine is, it's about finding the right drug. Placebo research begins to shift that whole concept." [Image: Pills via Shutterstock] [2328079-photo-schiller.jpg] Ben Schiller Ben Schiller is a New York-based staff writer for Co.Exist, and also contributes to the FT and Yale e360. He used to edit a European management magazine, and worked as a reporter in San Francisco, Prague and Brussels Continued August 26, 2013 | 9:15 AM Co.Exist World changing ideas and innovation Editor: Morgan Clendaniel Subscribe to Newsletters Enter Your Email: ____________________ submit EDITOR'S PICKS * [3023452-poster-p-intersection1.jpg] This Is What New York Looks Like From The Edge Of A Skyscraper * [3023128-poster-1280-ted.jpg] This Sleek Spiderman Spacesuit Could Take Astronauts To Mars * [3023190-poster-p-text.jpg] Why This Designer Writes Text Messages By Hand And Sends Them As Images EXPERT PERSPECTIVES * [3021147-poster-1280-sharing.jpg] It's Time For The Sharing Economy To Become The Sharing Society * [3021484-poster-1280-waves.jpg] 5 Tips From Social Intrapreneurs On How To Navigate Complex Corporate Politics * [3021539-poster-1280-connections.jpg] The Future Of Branding Is Creating Real Connections Between Consumers And Products From the Editor Thanks for coming to Co.Exist. This site is focused on groundbreaking innovation, innovation that’s going to change the way we live and the resources we use. We’re for brash and creative solutions, that make everyone rich while helping the people of the world lead lovely, clean, and fulfilling lives. Continued Contributors * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * Trending Articles * Advertise * Privacy Policy * Terms * About Us __________________________________________________________________ Fast Company & Inc © 2013 Mansueto Ventures, LLC. [p?c1=2&c2=6916907&cv=2.0&cj=1] § Patient.co.uk - Trusted medical information and support Skip to content * Patient Access * Home * Wellbeing * Health Information Health information for patients Search health information ____________________ Search A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z Diseases and Conditions + Arthritic / Bone / Muscle + Blood / Allergy + Brain / Nerves / Neurology + Cancer + Chest / Lung + Diabetes / Hormone + Ear / Nose / Throat / Mouth + Eyes + First Aid + Genito-urinary / Kidney + Gut / Bowel / Stomach + Heart / Blood Vessels + Infections + Injury / Accidents + Liver / Gallbladder + Mental Health + Sexual Health + Skin / Nail Disorders + Symptom information + Symptom checker Treatments and information + Anatomy / Body + Benefits / Social + Contraception + Health Promotion + Immunisation + Medications + Surgical Procedures + Tests / Investigations + Pregnancy and Newborns + Child Health + Teenage Health + Women's Health + Men's Health + Senior Health * Medicines Medicines and drugs Search health information ____________________ Search A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z Medicines used for: + Allergies + Blood / Nutrition Disorders + Breathing Problems + Cancer + Deficiencies + Ear / Nose / Throat / Mouth + Endocrine Disorders + Eye Conditions + Gastrointestinal Conditions + The Genito-urinary System + Heart Problems + HRT + The Immune System + Immunisation + Infections + Inflammation + Kidney Conditions + Metabolic Disorders + Muscles / Joints + Nausea / Vomiting + The Nervous System + Pain + The Reproductive System + Skin Conditions + Substance Misuse * Professional Reference PatientPlus Search our database of PatientPlus medical articles written by our expert medical authoring team. Professional reference Search professional reference ____________________ Search A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z UK clinical guidelines Search our database of links to UK clinical guidelines - including NICE, SIGN, GAIN, and Professional Colleges. UK Clinical Guidelines Search UK clinical guidelines ____________________ Search A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z Other resources + PatientPro + Medical calculators + Images and pictures + Medical Students + New clinical leaflets * Forums * Directory * Blogs * Patient Access [px.gif] Sign in [px.gif] Register Welcome to Patient.co.uk Register * Patient Access * Patient.co.uk * - MyHealth * - Forums * - PatientPro | Sign in * Patient Access * Patient.co.uk * - MyHealth * - Forums * - PatientPro MyHealth | Blogs | Shop | Symptom checker Search Patient.co.uk ____________________ Search 1. Home 2. Professional Reference 3. Placebo Effect Placebo Effect * * Tweet * Our resources on Placebo Effect * This article Article * more Discuss Discuss * Printable version Print * Download as PDF PDF * Email this article to a friend Email * [px.gif] Bookmark Bookmarked Remove? Cancel You must be signed in to bookmark pages View All * [px.gif] Notes Cancel You must be signed into your pro account to make notes * Listen to article Listen * Please wait... Translate This PatientPlus article is written for healthcare professionals so the language may be more technical than the condition leaflets. You may find the abbreviations list helpful. This page has been archived. It has not been updated since 22/06/2011. External links and references may no longer work. The placebo effect is a poorly understood phenomenon but recent work suggests that placebo represents the psychosocial aspect of every treatment and "the study of placebo is essentially the study of psychosocial context that surrounds the patient".^[1] Research in the field of neurobiology suggests that a person's expectation of an effect generates activity in certain areas of the brain. Brain regions identified as having activity after administration of a placebo include the anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia.^[2] The placebo effect needs to be taken into account in clinical trials: * It has been reported to affect quality-of-life assessments.^[3] * In certain settings placebo interventions can influence patient-reported outcomes, especially pain and nausea, although it is difficult to distinguish patient-reported effects of placebo from biased reporting. Variations in the effect of placebo were partly explained by variations in how trials were conducted and how patients were informed.^[4] It is important therefore to differentiate between the placebo effect exhibited in clinical trials (which needs to be minimised) and that seen in clinical practice (which should be maximised).^[5] The difference between a perceived placebo effect and a true placebo effect should be recognised. Save time & improve your PDP on Patient.co.uk * Notes Add notes to any clinical page and create a reflective diary * Track Automatically track and log every page you have viewed * Print Print and export a summary to use in your appraisal Click to find out more » Perceived placebo effect This is seen in the placebo arm of a clinical trial, eg new antihypertensive drugs may drop the average blood pressure by 5 mm Hg, but this may not be true placebo effect as other factors may confound the situation. Such factors include: * The natural course of the disease - blood pressure may normally reduce over time, thus exaggerating the placebo effect. * Regression towards the mean - biological variables often fluctuate.^[6] When first measured they are probably approaching their maximum and so further measurements are likely to show a reduction. * Increased skill of the investigator - this may alter measurements up or down as more measurements are made. * Variable factors within the same patient - white coat hypertension may reduce as patients become accustomed to having their blood pressure measured, which would increase the apparent placebo effect. * Non-apparent simultaneous changes - on entering the trial, the patient may change their behaviour, on purpose or otherwise, in such as way as to confound the outcome, eg eat less salt, take more exercise. True placebo effect This can only be studied if an untreated group is included along with active and placebo treated groups. These are relatively uncommon but it has been discovered that: * Placebo treatment is more effective in relieving pain compared with no treatment. To achieve this, patients need to be conscious (placebo was given to sleeping patients and no difference noted). * Physical placebos, eg invasive techniques such as injection, are more powerful than simple oral placebos.^[7] * Topical placebo is also more effective than oral placebo, eg in primary varicose veins. * There is no standard degree of placebo effect (approximately 30% is often given) and its extent depends upon numerous factors including: + The demeanour of the person offering treatment. + The patient's attitude to health and their feelings about the treatment and person offering it. + The suggestibility of the patient. + The form of treatment, eg whether it has worked before, how expensive it is, its invasiveness and the reasons given as to how it works. * Placebo effects on pain are generally greater than on other symptoms. * Placebo effects are not always useful and may produce adverse effects (nocebo effects). * Variations in placebo remission and response rates reported in randomised controlled trials can be significantly influenced by the country in which the trial is conducted.^[8] * A study of osteoarthritis patients elicited that the placebo effect was effective in reducing pain and stiffness and improving function.The pain-relieving effect increased when the active treatment effect, baseline pain and sample size increased and when placebo was given through injections/needles.^[9] * One study found no difference in placebo effect whether the patient was suffering from functional or organic gastrointestinal disease.^[10] * A study of migraine patients found that placebos were more effective in treating acute headache than in prophylaxis.^[11] Provide Feedback Further reading & references 1. Koshi EB, Short CA; Placebo theory and its implications for research and clinical practice: a review Pain Pract. 2007 Mar;7(1):4-20. 2. Oken BS; Placebo effects: clinical aspects and neurobiology. Brain. 2008 Nov;131(Pt 11):2812-23. Epub 2008 Jun 21. 3. Eickhoff JC; Placebo effect-adjusted assessment of quality of life in placebo-controlled clinical trials. Stat Med. 2008 Apr 30;27(9):1387-402. 4. Hrobjartsson A, Gotzsche PC; Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010 Jan 20;(1):CD003974. 5. Ernst E; Placebo: new insights into an old enigma. Drug Discov Today. 2007 May;12(9-10):413-8. Epub 2007 Apr 2. 6. Asmar R, Safar M, Queneau P; Evaluation of the placebo effect and reproducibility of blood pressure measurement in hypertension. Am J Hypertens. 2001 Jun;14(6 Pt 1):546-52. 7. Diener HC; Placebo effects in treating migraine and other headaches. Curr Opin Investig Drugs. 2010 Jul;11(7):735-9. 8. Garud S, Brown A, Cheifetz A, et al; Meta-analysis of the placebo response in ulcerative colitis. Dig Dis Sci. 2008 Apr;53(4):875-91. Epub 2007 Oct 13. 9. Zhang W, Robertson J, Jones AC, et al; The placebo effect and its determinants in osteoarthritis: meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Ann Rheum Dis. 2008 Dec;67(12):1716-23. Epub 2008 Jun 9. 10. Musial F, Klosterhalfen S, Enck P; Placebo responses in patients with gastrointestinal disorders. World J Gastroenterol. 2007 Jul 7;13(25):3425-9. 11. Diener HC, Schorn CF, Bingel U, et al; The importance of placebo in headache research. Cephalalgia. 2008 Oct;28(10):1003-11. Epub 2008 Aug 22. Disclaimer: This article is for information only and should not be used for the diagnosis or treatment of medical conditions. EMIS has used all reasonable care in compiling the information but make no warranty as to its accuracy. Consult a doctor or other health care professional for diagnosis and treatment of medical conditions. For details see our conditions. Original Author: Dr Laurence Knott Current Version: Dr Hayley Willacy Last Checked: 22/06/2011 Document ID: 2857 Version: 22 © EMIS Professional reference If you want more in-depth information, see our related articles aimed at health professionals: * Guidelines Guidelines Search over 2,000 articles aimed at health professionals Search professional reference ____________________ Search Advertise with us >> Professional - Doctor Shaun O’Hanlon and guests write about issues affecting the health industry today Latest posts * Should I intercalate? * A GP's Twelve Days of Christmas * "Doctor, doctor … I've become invisible!" * Overweight and obese - a heavy issue? * Consequences of consumerism Read more » Education Revise and test your medical knowledge with more than 15 articles, OSCEs & quizzes designed specifically for students. * Contraception - A general overview * Stable Angina * Cirrhosis * Hypertension * Diabetes mellitus See more » Subscribe to our mailing list * indicates required Email Address * ____________________ Are you a * * (*) patient * ( ) health professional Subscribe Mobile Patient With our new free app you can search our database of information, find your local hospital, dentist, pharmacies and more! See more of our apps » [twitter-bird.png] Patient.co.uk on Twitter Tweets by @patientuk follow us on twitter Disclaimer: Patient.co.uk has no control over the content of any external links. Inclusion does not imply endorsement by Patient.co.uk. Symptom checker tool Assess your symptoms online with this handy tool created by NHS Direct and receive feedback on your best course of action. Choose a symptom » Patient.co.uk is one of the most trusted medical resources in the UK, supplying evidence based information on a wide range of medical and health topics to patients and health professionals. Search Patient.co.uk ____________________ Search Connect with us * Twitter * Pinterest * Youtube * Google+ * Facebook For Patients * Health Information * Medicines * Clinical Trials * Wellbeing * MyHealth * Media * Directory * Patient Local * Patient Access Secure * Discussion Forums * Sarah Says... * George Ford For Health Professionals * Patient Plus * UK Clinical Guidelines * Evidence Based Medicine * Textbooks and Journals * News * Higher Education * Healthcare pro blog * Medical Calculators * Shared decision aids * DVLA * Medline Information About Patient.co.uk * Accessibility * Awards * Careers * Contact / Feedback * Cookie policy * Disclaimer / Terms * Editorial Policy * FAQs * Mobile Access * Search widget * Site Map About Us| Media Enquiries| Press Releases| Commercial opportunities| Advertise with us Hitwise top 10 website Website of the year 2013 Royal sociey for public health 2012 BMA Patient Information Awards winners The Information Standard Hon Code Certified Egton Medical Information Systems Limited. Registered in England. No 2117205 Registered Office: Rawdon House, Green Lane, Yeadon, Leeds LS19 7BY § * Login * Register Menu Search Search this site: _______________ Search Leave this field blank: ____________________ * Home * Overview * Research * Foundations * Academic * Write Paper * For Kids * Forum * Login * Register * Home * Overview * Research * Foundations * Academic * Write Paper * For Kids * Forum Home > Research > Experiments > Placebo Effect Placebo Effect * Home * Overview * Research + Basics + Methods + Experiments + Design + Statistics * Foundations + Overview + Reasoning + Philosophy + Ethics + History * Academic + Fields + Psychology + Biology + Physics + Medicine * Write Paper * For Kids * Forum Explorable.com Printer-friendly version Send by email PDF version Share this page on your website Share this page on your website: Pla __________________________________________________ A placebo effect is a phenomenon which may result in some therapeutic effect in subjects given placebo. Experimental Research * 1Experimental Research * 2Research Variables + 2.1Independent Variable + 2.2Dependent Variable + 2.3Controlled Variables + 2.4Third Variable * 3Cause and Effect + 3.1Control Group + 3.2Research Bias + 3.3Social Science Subjects o 3.3.1Placebo Effect o 3.3.2Double Blind Method * 4Conducting an Experiment + 4.1Randomized Controlled Trials + 4.2Pretest-Posttest + 4.3Solomon Four Group + 4.4Between Subjects + 4.5Within Subject + 4.6Repeated Measures + 4.7Counterbalanced Measures + 4.8Matched Subjects [3.3.1 Placebo Effect____________] Your email address: * ________________________________________ Take this as a course This article is a part of the course "Experimental Research". Enter your email to take the full course. Leave this field blank: ____________________ Get the offline version of this guide Kindle - $2.99 iBooks - $2.99 PDF - $2.99 ePub-format - $2.99 A placebo is a sham medical intervention that exploits the hope/expectancy-effect to the treatment. It involves giving subject an inert sugar pill, and he/she is told that it may improve the condition, but the person is completely unaware of the fact that it is inert. In medicine, it has been observed that patient’s belief does indeed sometimes result in therapeutic effect and cause patient’s condition to improve. Placebo treatment is widely practices in medicine, and it has been recognized to have clinically important effects. A placebo has been defined as a substance or any procedure that is given objectively without any specific activity for the condition being treated. Indeed, wide variety of things can be considered placebo and do show placebo effect. These may include pharmacological substances like pills, inhalants, creams, injections or even medical devices such as ultrasound, surgery, acupuncture to name few. In one study, it was observed that patient recovery can be enhanced with positive suggestions and comments by the physician like “you will certainly be better in few days with this treatment” and if the patient is given negative words like “I am not sure that this treatment can work for you” it negatively affect the recovery of patient. How it Works? The placebo effect phenomenon is related to the perception and expectations of the subject. If subject see it helpful, it can heal, and if viewed as harmful, it can cause negative effects or what is known as nocebo effect. Both expectations and conditioning play an important role in placebo effect and make different kind of contribution. The expectancy effect is seen to be enhanced through factors like positive attitude of doctor, different in color and size of placebo pills, or use of injections. In one study, researchers found that response to placebo treatment was increased from 44% to 62% when the healthcare professional gave them warmth, attention, and confidence. Conditioning on the other hand exerts long lasting effects and it seems to affect early stages of information processing. However, as placebo effect is based upon conditioning and expectations of the subject, the effect is seen to disappear if the subject is informed that his/her expectations are unrealistic, or that placebo treatment is completely ineffective. Despite ongoing controversy concerning use of placebo therapy, there are number of reports showing positive effects of placebo treatment. Placebo seems to work in alleviating pain, anxiety, depression, Parkinson’s disease, arthritis, and even cancer. Remarkable recovery of “Mr. Wright” from a deadly cancer is just an example of placebo treatment. Placebo effects seems not only be arise from conscious belief but also from subconscious associations between recovery and treatment as researchers has found that placebo effect does arise from active processes in brain. « Previous Social Science Subjects Back to Overview Experimental Research Next » Double Blind Method We would love feedback on this article. Please let us know about any error. We highly appreciate suggestions. Suggest changes View the discussion thread. Share this page on your website: Pla __________________________________________________ Get the offline version of this guide Kindle - $2.99 iBooks - $2.99 PDF - $2.99 ePub-format - $2.99 Follow Explorable on Twitter Tweets about "@ExplorableMind OR explorable.com" Subscribe to Newsletter Sign up for our newsletter now! Languages * Español [INS: :INS] Footer bottom Links * About * FAQ * Privacy Policy * Contact * Site Map Website Explorable.com is centered around the scientific method, psychology, research and academia. Thank you to... Innoventus Innovation Norway The Research Council of Norway Design by Gilli Subscribe / Share * Subscribe to our RSS Feed * Pin us on Pinterest * Oskar Blakstad (Founder) on Twitter Explorable.com - Copyright © 2008-2013. All Rights Reserved. Wilson Mizner: "If you steal from one author it's plagiarism; if you steal from many it's research." Don't steal, do research. drupal stats § #RSS Less Wrong A community blog devoted to refining the art of human rationality Future of Humanity Institute Machine Intelligence Research Institute Center for Applied Rationality * Main + Posts + Comments * Discussion + Posts + Comments Less Wrong is a community blog devoted to refining the art of human rationality. Please visit our About page for more information. The Anti-Placebo Effect 39 Post author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 05:44AM Just about everyone is familiar with the placebo effect at this point. What I've discovered through my personal studies working with people suffering from anxiety and depression is that there is actually a significant related effect, which I have dubbed the anti-placebo effect. Google says: pla·ce·bo ef·fect noun 1. a beneficial effect, produced by a placebo drug or treatment, that cannot be attributed to the properties of the placebo itself, and must therefore be due to the patient's belief in that treatment. I say: anti·pla·ce·bo ef·fect noun 1. 1. a beneficial effect, produced by a treatment, that is not attributed to treatment itself or has stopped being noticed, and thus the patient does not believe in that treatment as effective. Its easy to miss treatment working. For example, as a kid grows up, its easy to miss how their vocabulary is growing, but for someone who doesn't seem them every day, it may be immediately obvious "my how they're talking more!" In other words, an anti-placebo effect is what happens when someone is having an intervention that is causing their life to improve, but the person does not believe that they are improving. This effect is most common with people who suffer from depression, who have biases for sad [1] and otherwise negative stimuli compared to the general population, and is also true of people suffering from anxiety from my personal client tracking. Its also important to note that this bias persists after the recovery of the depressive episode. The reason that this is important is that those recovering from anxiety and depression have a tendency to believe that they are not doing as well as they are - due to this cognitive bias creating an anti-placebo effect for them, which results in their giving up too soon on interventions which are effective and thus not getting better and regressing to old unpleasant patterns. It has been interesting since tracking results of my own clients [2] - I have all of them track scores at the beginning of their sessions on the site moodscope.com at the beginning of their sessions, so that we can see their progress over time with a consistent bias of the time of tracking being start of session (as opposed to other random biases such as wanting to take the quiz when in an especially good or bad mood). I also take extensive notes and track other metrics of progress. What I've found, is that many clients hit a point after a few weeks or months, where they are questioning if they have made any progress. Because I take metrics to prepare for this, I am able to point my clients at their metrics, and say for example, that according to their self reports, their mood has increased by 50% and their productivity has doubled. What typically happens when I review score + notes with the client in question is that once they look back at how things were before compared to how they are now, they realize that they actually have made progress, and this is often followed with additional forward progress. It is interesting to put this in perspective with the hedonic treadmill [3]. The hedonic treadmill is the supposed tendency of humans to quickly return to a relatively stable level of happiness despite major positive or negative events or life changes. What I'm finding with my studies is that it is often true for people recovering from depression when they take an overall evaluation (go meta), especially from a low point, but that when they look back at the factors that have changed, and they take the mood score test looking at different aspects of their experience on moodscope.com, they actually do have a more positive life experience when measured this way. When I point out the inconsistency, people generally determine that the moodscope.com reported experience is more accurate (especially when supplemented by going over session notes) and over time, most clients do get off the hedonic treadmill and proceed to having the meta level catch up with moodscope.com. The good news about this for people suffering from anxiety and depression at large: If you are aware of the negative cognitive bias and anti-placebo effect, you can take steps to account for and correct this bias. One of the best ways to do this is by taking metrics along with notes that you can look at later. When you look back, look at what your overall trend is, and try to focus on that more than if you happened to have a bad day or week. If you have been progressing with a good linear regression, odds are that if you don't give up the new better patterns and habits you have created, they will continue to serve you. Although external factors to the one variable you are studying do complicate this and need to be taken into account. [1] http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2847035/ [2] http://www.depressiontoproductivity.com/your-clients-really-improve / [3] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hedonic_treadmill Article Navigation Loading… Vote up Vote down Comments (39) * CC Licenced Comments (39) Sort By: Best Best Popular New Controversial Top Old Leading Load all comments Comment author: TheOtherDave 28 September 2013 04:09:13PM * 28 points [+] (3 children) Comment author: TheOtherDave 28 September 2013 04:09:13PM * 28 points [-] The thing that made this most obvious to me was training my dog. For example, when conditioning her out of a behavior I dislike, my intuitive approach seems to be to monitor the intensity of the behavior... I expect a steady stream of intense performances BBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBB to become a stream of less-intense performances bbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbb and then disappear altogether. But in reality the intensity doesn't change much, and sometimes it even gets worse. I never get "bbb..." What declines is the frequency. That is, BBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBBB becomes BBBBBB_BBBBBBBBB_BBB which, from my intuitive perspective, is essentially no progress at all... I don't notice. And then on the other end, when the behavior is almost extinguished, I get _____B___B_______B___ which, from my perspective, is an endless series of "oh good, we got rid of this behavior -- oh, crap, now it's back again, just as bad as it ever was! This isn't working!" So it really really helped to keep a chart of the frequency of the behavior that I can look back at and realize that actually, we've been making steady progress all along, even though it completely doesn't feel like progress at all. After my stroke, when I was relearning to walk/talk/think, that strategy was absolutely critical to warding off despair. I charted everything, and it made a huge difference. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: hyporational 29 September 2013 10:19:12AM 4 points [+] (2 children) Comment author: hyporational 29 September 2013 10:19:12AM 4 points [-] After your stroke, what kinds of things did you chart specifically? In which areas was the progress most motivating? * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: TheOtherDave 29 September 2013 03:33:08PM 9 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: TheOtherDave 29 September 2013 03:33:08PM 9 points [-] Some examples: How many times I could raise and lower my leg before being exhausted. Number of steps I could take at a time. Words I blocked on. Number of times I became altogether unable to complete a sentence. (Though this was more approximate.) Number of pushups I could do. Distance I could walk. Complexity of the hardest logic-puzzle I'd successfully solved. How many words I could write before my hand was exhausted. How coherently I could write (I still have somewhere a piece of paper on which I wrote the same sentence, over and over, on each line; it gradually morphs from a literally indecipherable scrawl to something indistinguishable from a normal-though-sloppy person's handwriting.) These were, of course, at different times in my recovery. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: TheOtherDave 29 September 2013 04:01:46PM 6 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: TheOtherDave 29 September 2013 04:01:46PM 6 points [-] Oh, and re: motivating... that's a much more difficult question to answer. The charting techniques were "motivating" only in a prophylactic sense... they helped me resist a certain kind of "I'm not getting any better" despair that was otherwise very compelling and very demotivating. Positive motivation to progress was much harder to come by, and I had a much less concrete grasp on it. I was often in a not quite apathetic, but highly disengaged state with respect to my recovery. Mostly I dealt with this by accepting it as just another intermittent deficit where I had to ride out the bad periods and take advantage of the good ones. I think the closest I can come to describing it accurately is to say that motivation-to-progress was highly correlated with focus; when what I was doing was recovery, I was very motivated to make progress. What direction causality ran, though, I have no idea. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: pjeby 30 September 2013 10:06:46PM 7 points [+] (3 children) Comment author: pjeby 30 September 2013 10:06:46PM 7 points [-] A related concept: "change amnesia". Our general memory of our past behaviors is heavily influenced by our expected present behaviors, such that we tend not to remember how we behaved differently in the past, and have difficulty believing we were ever that dumb/lame/whatever when actually confronted with evidence (such as pictures, journal entries, etc.) Some people have this worse than others: a couple times I've had clients who came to me with a large stack of personal problems, and three months later don't know why they're paying me because they're not getting any changes... and then when I ask about the problems they had when they started, they usually remember telling me about those problems, but don't remember having the problems any more. (As in, they don't remember experiencing the problem, and don't think that they would be the kind of person who would've had a problem like that!) For me personally, I have a lot of trouble reconstructing how I thought about a subject, once I've self-modified on that topic, with the effect usually setting in after a few hours or overnight. (Although honestly, I've never tried such a reconstruction any sooner; the difficulty might set in within minutes for all I know.) So I can remember that I used to think in some messed-up way, and maybe even what behaviors resulted from that messed-up thinking, but actually simulating the previous mode of thinking becomes difficult and unnatural. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: hyporational 01 October 2013 03:27:50PM * 1 point [+] (2 children) Comment author: hyporational 01 October 2013 03:27:50PM * 1 point [-] The few times I tried, I had to quit journaling out of pure embarrassment. After a few months, pretty much the only reaction I had reading old entries was: "who the hell is this idiot?" My past self was completely alien to me, and I couldn't even understand what the words meant. I didn't really write for comprehension, as I thought my future self would totally understand me. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Kawoomba 01 October 2013 03:52:31PM 4 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: Kawoomba 01 October 2013 03:52:31PM 4 points [-] Makes you wonder how crucial that whole value-preservation thing actually is ... * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: hyporational 01 October 2013 04:13:05PM * 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: hyporational 01 October 2013 04:13:05PM * 0 points [-] Hehe, a good point. I don't have such a strong reaction reading old forum posts for example, though. At least I can understand them, even if I don't agree with them. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Shield 28 September 2013 11:22:18AM 6 points [+] (8 children) Comment author: Shield 28 September 2013 11:22:18AM 6 points [-] Are you sure that "anti placebo effect" is a good name though? The placebo effect refers exclusively to medical treatment if I'm not entirely mistaken, and this seems to have much broader implications in basically any sort of training. It's still basically the same effect if someone refuses to notice the progress they made with say tutoring, but it has nothing to do with medicine or treatment. Seems a bit misleading. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: timtyler 29 September 2013 08:59:16AM * 4 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: timtyler 29 September 2013 08:59:16AM * 4 points [-] Are you sure that "anti placebo effect" is a good name though? It may be that nocebo has a better claim to being an "anti-placebo effect". * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Vaniver 28 September 2013 05:26:59PM * 3 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: Vaniver 28 September 2013 05:26:59PM * 3 points [-] The placebo effect refers exclusively to medical treatment if I'm not entirely mistaken, and this seems to have much broader implications in basically any sort of training. While the placebo effect is generally defined in a medical context, people rarely throw type errors when you talk about placebos outside of medicine. The Hawthorne Effect is the name that productivity boosts due to observation / novelty / active treatment go by, but it's seen as similar to if not the same as the placebo effect. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Desrtopa 29 September 2013 04:27:37PM 1 point [+] (0 children) Comment author: Desrtopa 29 September 2013 04:27:37PM 1 point [-] I can affirm that I expected the term to refer to something different, namely an effect that causes people to do worse based on their concerns surrounding an intervention, such as in this study on intercessory prayer, where the individuals who knew they were being prayed for did worse than the other groups, possibly because they were concerned that they were so badly off that they needed people to pray for them. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Lethalmud 30 September 2013 12:33:17PM 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: Lethalmud 30 September 2013 12:33:17PM 0 points [-] Maybe Nocebo? http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Nocebo * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: ShannonFriedman 30 September 2013 06:37:53AM 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: ShannonFriedman 30 September 2013 06:37:53AM 0 points [-] Here's my answer to that: http://lesswrong.com/lw/iqr/theantiplaceboeffect/9tiw * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Zaine 29 September 2013 02:04:31PM 0 points [+] (2 children) Comment author: Zaine 29 September 2013 02:04:31PM 0 points [-] In the placebo effect, you try something, see results, and believe those results derived from what you tried, when in fact what you tried could not possibly have had any effect whatsoever; the observed results are then attributed to one's beliefs that the tried thing had the capacity for effecting change. The above refers to a different phenomenon: one tries something, doesn't see results, and believes what they tried had no effect, when in fact what they tried did have results. In the placebo effect, one's beliefs effect change. In the phenomenon Shannon refers to, change occurs regardless of one's beliefs. Interestingly, when I presented the above description of the placebo effect to someone and asked for what they would expect of the opposite, they replied, "Change happens and they don't believe it." I would think the term, "Opposite-Placebo Effect" or "Opposite of the Placebo Effect" a better descriptor, as 'anti-' implies simple negation rather than a flipping of observed effects. This could of course just be an issue of differing perspectives on what is or isn't an intuitive moniker. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: kalium 29 September 2013 08:44:54PM 4 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: kalium 29 September 2013 08:44:54PM 4 points [-] Actually the placebo effect is a statistical term covering the entire improvement seen in the placebo branch of a trial. Part of the effect comes from beliefs, yes. But there are other causes. For instance, people tend to enroll in clinical trials when their health is at a local minimum, and reversion to the mean can account for a good chunk of their improvement. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Zaine 30 September 2013 05:06:03PM * 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: Zaine 30 September 2013 05:06:03PM * 0 points [-] I didn't know this, and if there are other instances, would like to know all of them. Thank you! * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Viliam_Bur 28 September 2013 07:24:31AM * 4 points [+] (2 children) Comment author: Viliam_Bur 28 September 2013 07:24:31AM * 4 points [-] I have noticed this kind of effect on myself -- I start doing something... the improvement is very slow... I convince myself that the improvement is zero... so I stop doing it... so I never finish it... which I take as a proof that my improvement was zero. Having unrealistic expectations makes it even worse, because the higher expectation the more disappointment with slow progress. Probably the correct solution would be: just record your improvements. I imagine something like beeminder, just without the deadlines: create a graph for something, enter data points, see the visualization, celebrate when you reach some goal. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: 4hodmt 28 September 2013 07:46:20AM 11 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: 4hodmt 28 September 2013 07:46:20AM 11 points [-] Beeminder without the deadlines is Beeminder with the weekly rate set to zero. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: army1987 28 September 2013 10:41:05PM 2 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: army1987 28 September 2013 10:41:05PM 2 points [-] Or even just a text file and a gnuplot script, for that matter. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: hyporational 28 September 2013 06:12:56AM * 4 points [+] (3 children) Comment author: hyporational 28 September 2013 06:12:56AM * 4 points [-] I've had personal experience with this phenomenom, but never associated it with the hedonic treadmill. I think this has also something to do with you current emotional state determining which episodic memories you recall. When you feel good, you remember all the good stuff, when you feel bad, all the bad things pile up. I've found it's really difficult to remember what it was like being depressed when I'm well. That has lead to stupidly quitting medication a couple of times. Is there a name for this effect? EDIT: In my case the improvement with medication was dramatic, so it wasn't just a case of not noticing slow progress. My point was the anti-placebo effect probably consists of many different kinds of known cognitive failure modes, and it might be helpful to recognize them. In the problem that I describe, the difficulty is in retrieving memories that don't fit the emotional context. The hedonic treadmill effect is another type of an effect that fits the category. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 08:22:44PM * 2 points [+] (2 children) Comment author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 08:22:44PM * 2 points [-] What you're describing sounds like the results from the anti-placebo effect, although I didn't go so far as naming that. Basically, you don't realize its working (anti-placebo effect), and then you stop and regress (what you're pointing to). Since you've figured this out, you should have a much easier time avoiding it with the next intervention you try, especially if you track the metrics you're most interested in seeing changes in. One place that things get tricky is that your negative reinforcement loops can get started while you're still tracking metrics - as an example, perhaps you're doing great, and then you have one bad day, and then you make the false assumption that the bad day means that you will continue having bad days and that none of the other progress is real. If you adopt that belief, then even your mood tracking will decline, so its important to be reality checking as much as possible along the way, and to remind yourself that one bad day is not as big a deal as weeks of good days, and that will help you stay on track. Here's a video of me role playing the two attitudes that might be helpful. Not quite on topic, but same principle - replace getting the work task done with having succeeded in improving on the metric you've been tracking for several consecutive weeks, and imagine how the optimistic person would respond to a down day, with that attitude, v.s. the overwhelmed/depressed role play person. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: hyporational 29 September 2013 09:58:01AM * 0 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: hyporational 29 September 2013 09:58:01AM * 0 points [-] Edited the original to clarify. The video link didn't work, it's here, pretty descriptive of the basic attitudes. Changing attitudes to me feels like changing beliefs, you can't simply choose them by tricks of imagination. Medication helped with that. These days my problem is more simple avolition than depression, any non pharma tips on that? (I'm a physician.) It would be nice to know how much of imagined SSRI poop out doesn't actually happen. Unfortunately there aren't similar tracking tools in finnish to give to my patients. Maybe pen and paper should suffice. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: ShannonFriedman 29 September 2013 02:47:57PM 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: ShannonFriedman 29 September 2013 02:47:57PM 0 points [-] Yes, a lot of it has do to with having the parts of your mind that are action oriented not in alignment with the ones that want to have fun. If you're in that state for a long time, or in any way that ingrains the patterns hard, eventually you get to more extreme states like lack of interest in anything. I use non-medication techniques to get people out of these states as my profession. You might find this audio helpful. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: RolfAndreassen 28 September 2013 06:17:07PM 3 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: RolfAndreassen 28 September 2013 06:17:07PM 3 points [-] people suffering from anxiety from my personal client tracking. This phrase probably does not carry the intended meaning. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 08:14:34PM 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 08:14:34PM 0 points [-] LOL, indeed :) * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: CronoDAS 30 September 2013 07:04:39AM 2 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: CronoDAS 30 September 2013 07:04:39AM 2 points [-] I'm reminded of the peak-end heuristic; the times that you're miserable are often far more dramatic and available than the times that you're not, so when you think of how you've been feeling over a period of time, you get a biased result. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: katydee 28 September 2013 08:43:04AM 2 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: katydee 28 September 2013 08:43:04AM 2 points [-] Very interesting post. I'd like to hear more about what sorts of situations tend to produce these effects, as it sounds like information that could potentially be quite valuable. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: EvelynM 30 September 2013 02:28:55AM 1 point [+] (1 child) Comment author: EvelynM 30 September 2013 02:28:55AM 1 point [-] This effect isn't limited to people with anxiety and depression. People losing weight also experience it: http://www.3fatchicks.com/forum/metabolic-research-center/146593-paper- towel-theory.html It's hard to be objective about yourself. Using tools to gain objectivity is a good thing. I agree anti-placebo isn't a a good name. It's more like a distorted or cloudy mirror (don't see yourself clearly). but that doesn't capture the difficulty of remembering precisely how you were a long time before, to capture changes that happen at a slow rate. Having a good name for this, will help people to pass along the idea. Thanks Shannon. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: ShannonFriedman 30 September 2013 04:19:39AM * 2 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: ShannonFriedman 30 September 2013 04:19:39AM * 2 points [-] The way that I came up with the name, was that someone was suggesting that my taking metrics might create a placebo effect, where people would believe that they were doing better than they actually were. So, my response to this was that I was not trying to create a placebo effect, but rather, to avoid a placebo effect in the opposite direction. So while I agree with you that this effect is not an opposite (why I referred to it as related instead of reverse), I do think that it is the opposite of what a lot of people fear - that they are experiencing a placebo effect. In short, people being afraid of having a placebo effect is often how this effect comes to be - they don't want to create false hope and then have it dissipate, so instead they refuse to believe or acknowledge real positive results when they see them. So, I would say that the title is reasonable regarding people's expectations, but not in the precise using of the term placebo sense. Personally, I think that expectations are more important for titling. Fewer people will pay attention to a precisely named definition that they know nothing about, whereas calling it the anti-placebo effect grabs attention - specifically the attention of people who have this bias. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: hyporational 29 September 2013 10:28:35AM 1 point [+] (1 child) Comment author: hyporational 29 September 2013 10:28:35AM 1 point [-] I wonder if there are superstimuli for appreciating gradual progression that impair our sensitivity to do so. Video games come to mind. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Viliam_Bur 30 September 2013 08:15:56AM * 4 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: Viliam_Bur 30 September 2013 08:15:56AM * 4 points [-] The whole internet thing -- you click something and you expect to get results within seconds. Train your expectations with this... and then try to accomplish something in real life. Even if it takes days, it feels like eternity. If it takes months, it might as well happen after Singularity. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Dentin 28 September 2013 06:46:30AM 1 point [+] (0 children) Comment author: Dentin 28 September 2013 06:46:30AM 1 point [-] (Posting feedback as requested) Shannon - the only real problem I see in my sleep-deprived stupor is the very last sentence, which seems out of place and partially constructed. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Decius 11 October 2013 10:08:52AM 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: Decius 11 October 2013 10:08:52AM 0 points [-] Mindhacking idea: fudge the historical results such that the trend line always appears positive, and use that as a primary intervention. This requires either violating informed consent (a bad thing) or exercising doublethink or another method of self-deception. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: CronoDAS 30 September 2013 07:03:27AM * 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: CronoDAS 30 September 2013 07:03:27AM * 0 points [-] When I saw "anti-placebo effect" I thought of this instead... Edit: Beaten to it. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: tgb 28 September 2013 03:16:20PM 0 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: tgb 28 September 2013 03:16:20PM 0 points [-] Dark arts for a good use? Modify the surveys to show them having even more progress than they did so they get even more self-confidence. Don't worry, they won't notice! * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: Dorikka 29 September 2013 04:14:08AM 0 points [+] (0 children) Comment author: Dorikka 29 September 2013 04:14:08AM 0 points [-] Except that if one did, things might go very, very badly for you. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink Comment author: RomeoStevens 28 September 2013 08:38:05AM 0 points [+] (1 child) Comment author: RomeoStevens 28 September 2013 08:38:05AM 0 points [-] I have been supplementing rhodiola rosea and your example is the exact thing it promises, a halving of depressive symptoms along with other benefits. I'm not sure if it's working. * Vote up * Vote down * * Reply * Permalink Comment author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 03:23:58PM * 1 point [+] (0 children) Comment author: ShannonFriedman 28 September 2013 03:23:58PM * 1 point [-] Yeah, hard to know in retrospect, I would love to hear more about your results in as much as you can tell. For future such attempts where you're trying something like this, I recommend taking a baseline on a mood tracking site for a week or two before starting, if you can manage it, and then tracking for at least however long they claim it takes to get results. I also recommend just generally taking baselines, maybe every couple to few months - that way, even if you don't want to mood track all the time, you at least have some reasonable random sampling to look back on to see how you're doing over time. When you track, its ideal if you find a way to have uniform bias - so always take the test at the same time each day for example, or randomize the time if you're doing many data points and can handle noise - one problem with mood tracking is that people tend to take it when they are feeling especially good or especially bad or generally motivated by extreme mood, so its good if you can find a way to minimize that particular bias. * Vote up * Vote down * * Parent * Reply * Permalink * wiki * Sequences * About _______________________________ Search Register / Login ____________________ Password ____________________ Remember me [ ] Recover password (BUTTON) Login Feed icon Subscribe to RSS Feed Nearest Meetups Recent Comments Recent Rationality Quotes Recent Posts Recent Wiki Edits Top Contributors, 30 Days Recent Karma Awards Powered by Reddit Powered by Reddit * About Less Wrong * Report Issues You'll need to login or register to do that (Don't worry, it only takes a few seconds) Create Pick a username and password for your Less Wrong and Less Wrong Wiki accounts. You will receive an email to verify your account. * Username: ____________________ * Email: ____________________ * Password: ____________________ * Verify password: ____________________ * i wonder if these things even work ______________________________ * [ ] Remember me (BUTTON) Create account Login Already have an account and just want to login? * Username: ____________________ * Password: ____________________ * [ ] Remember me (BUTTON) Login Forgot your password? Close this window Close this window § #Alleydog Glossary RSS Glossary App for iOS and Android | Sign In | Become A Member * Home * Glossary * Members + Flashcards + Quizzes + Q&A + Guides + Sign Up + Login * Grad Schools * Psych Degrees * Class Notes * Psych Topics * Psych Jobs * Videos * More + Psych News + Psychology Jobs + Word of the Day + Psychology Journals + Psych Writing + Email + GRE + Psychology Articles + Psych Links + Get Into Grad School + Advertise + Support [dog.png] Psychology Glossary ____________________ [search.png]-Submit A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z Follow AlleyDog Placebo (placebo effect) __________________________________________________________________ Placebo (placebo effect): A placebo is any substance that is not known to have any pharmacological effects (produces no meaningful changes in an oranism, either chemical, biological, etc.) that is made to look like an active ("real") drug. Sometimes the act of taking a pill produces an effect if the person believes the pill is active. To compensate for this, scientists often give placebos to determine if an effect is due to the "real" drug or from the act of just taking a pill. For additional information, see Control Condition. IFRAME: http://www.alleydog.com/school/featured_widget/degree.php __________________________________________________________________ Get the Word Of The Day delivered to your inbox __________________________________________________________________ Glossary Quick Jump A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z (c) 1998-2013, AlleyDog.com. All material within this site is the property of AlleyDog.com. This material may not be reprinted or copied for any reason without the express written consent of AlleyDog.com. Quantcast Clicky § #next Skip to main content Welcome Guest | Login or Register Welcome My Account|My Account or Logout Connect to other sites within the UBM Medica Network Home Search For... ____________________________________________________________ Search [Psychiatric Times] Go Topics: ADHD|Bipolar Disorder|Blogs|Borderline Personality|Child Adolescent Psychiatry|Major Depressive Disorder|Personality Disorders|Schizophrenia|Suicide|Career|CME|Browse All Topics [All Topics_________________] Main menu * Home * Topics * Buyer's Guide * Journal * Home * Topics * Schizophrenia * Bipolar * ADHD * Depression * Conferences * Clinical Scales * Classifieds * Special Reports * Journal Understanding and Using the Placebo Effect Understanding and Using the Placebo Effect October 01, 2006 | Addiction, Alcohol Abuse, Amphetamine Related Disorders By Walter A. Brown, MD Most physicians make at least some use of the placebo effect to enhance treatments, whether they realize it or not. This article examines the extent of the placebo effect in patients with psychiatric illness, and reviews what is known about how placebos work. It then discusses the application of the placebo response in enhancing the effects of treatment modalities in psychiatry. It is instructive to note a feature of virtually every antidepressant efficacy trial: in outpatients with moderately severe depression whose symptoms are assessed using the Hamilton Rating Scale, most of the improvement occurs during the first 2 weeks of treatment, and during this time, no differences in outcome between active drug and placebo are evident. After about 2 weeks, the placebo response reaches a plateau. Response to medication may continue to increase for a time, but during the period when most of the improvement occurs, placebo and medication produce similar responses. In fact, the strength of the placebo response can be quite significant in several psychiatric disorders.^1 Panic disorder is highly responsive to placebo, with a nearly 50% improvement in symptoms among patients assigned to that treatment strategy. In patients with posttraumatic stress disorder or depression, the placebo response is greater than 30%, and a response in the 30% range is also seen in generalized anxiety disorder. Patients with other conditions, including obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and psychosis, are less likely to exhibit a placebo response. Clearly, the placebo response is not uniformly strong in all psychiatric conditions. We have used these findings as a basis for recommending that clinical trials of agents being tested in patients with psychiatric disorders always include a placebo arm.^1 Placebo vs pharmacotherapy or psychotherapy How do responses to placebo compare with those achieved with medication? When treated with clonazepam or an SSRI, 60% to 70% of patients with panic disorder become panic-free during a 10-week treatment period. During the same period, half or more of patients with panic disorder who are treated with placebo become panic-free, underscoring the magnitude and importance of the placebo effect in this common condition.^2Active drug treatment for depression leads to improvement in about 60% to 70% of patients, while placebo administration is followed by improvement in 30% to 40%, again an impressive contribution.^3 Conditions that are poorly responsive to psychotherapy are also unlikely to respond to placebo. Patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, for example, are more likely to improve with medication than with behavior therapy. We would not expect a robust response to placebo in this patient group. The results of a collaborative National Institute of Mental Health study on the treatment of depression are informative.^4 Most of the patients had unipolar disease and were mildly, moderately, or severely ill. Some were randomized to clinical management and placebo; they attended a clinic, were given a placebo pill, and asked how they were doing. Patients in other groups were randomly assigned to cognitive-behavioral therapy, interpersonal psychotherapy, or imipramine. When the results were analyzed, no differences in outcome were evident according to the various treatments. None showed any advantage over the others. The analysis was also conducted according to whether patients were mildly or moderately/severely ill, and there the effects of some of the treatments did separate. In the less severely ill patients, no statistically significant difference was present among the treatments. In the more severely ill patients, imipramine, which was a popular antidepressant at the time the study was conducted, had an advantage over the 2 psychotherapies and placebo. Although there was a difference between the psychotherapies and placebo, it was not statistically significant. In the severely ill patients, imipramine worked better than any other treatment modality, but placebo was about as good as the psychotherapeutic strategies.^4 Although cognitive therapy for depression is widely used and taught in residency programs, the overwhelming majority of studies that have compared cognitive therapy with a pill placebo in the treatment of moderately severe depression show no difference in efficacy.^5,6 Less severely ill patients may benefit from any one of several different types of treatment, including psychotherapy, alternative therapies, and placebo. The benefits of placebo treatment are, of course, not limited to psychiatry. In a review of hypertension therapy, it was documented that active pharmacologic agents were associated with a 40% to 60% rate of reducing blood pressure to the normal range, while placebo was associated with a 25% success rate.^7 Some neurologic conditions, asthma, and certain pain problems are also responsive to a placebo effect. What is a placebo response? In my opinion, the term placebo is misleading. In fact, patients who receive "placebo" treatment get much more than a sugar pill, whether they are enrolled in a placebo-controlled clinical trial or are the recipients of ordinary medical care. "Treatment situation" is a more evocative term than "placebo treatment," and it includes several essential elements associated with healing, as shown in Table 1. Table 1 Elements of the treatment situation Recognized healer Healing symbols Evaluation Healing rituals Diagnosis Prognosis Plausible treatment All these elements, including healing symbols and rituals, evaluations, and discussions of diagnosis and prognosis, have an important role and are part of the placebo treatment or treatment situation. Hippocrates said that physicians should make frequent visits with their patients, inquire into all particulars of their situations, and learn about prognosis. The goal was to inspire confidence among patients so that they would feel comfortable entrusting their care to the physician. As an example, consider how patients respond to diagnostic tests. In a 1981 paper that appeared in the Annals of Internal Medicine, clinical outcomes were assessed in a group of 176 patients who had chest pain that was considered nonspecific.^8 Participants in this study were randomized to have either routine ECG and serum creatine phosphokinase tests or to have no diagnostic tests. Short-term disability was reported by fewer patients (20%) in the test group than in the patients who received no tests (46%); this difference was statistically significant. Diagnostic tests were an independent predictor of recovery. More than half (57%) of the patients in the test group felt that the care they received was "better than usual." Fewer than one third of the patients who did not receive tests felt this way.^8 This is evidence that doing some simple testing often has an impact on how people feel. Similarly, a study conducted in New Zealand suggests the importance of a formality such as writing on a prescription pad. A group of 456 sedentary patients were given verbal recommendations to increase their physical activity levels. Then they were randomized to a written exercise prescription or to a verbal advice group. The number of people in both groups who engaged in physical activity increased markedly after 6 weeks, but more participants in the written prescription than the verbal advice group were active. The investigators concluded that a written exercise prescription was an important adjunct in getting their patients to increase their physical activity levels.^9 In our culture, the written prescription has a large symbolic impact. Placebo response vs placebo effect No doubt some of the improvements observed in placebo-treated patients occur simply because of the passage of time. Many of the conditions that are most placebo-responsive, including insomnia, pain, asthma, hypertension, and depression, are by nature fluctuating illnesses. Some of the improvement we see associated with placebo is a result of regression to the mean. Nonethe- less, considerable evidence suggests that placebo treatment actually adds something that goes beyond the mere passage of time. Some would propose that the placebo effect should be defined as the placebo response minus the no-treatment response, or minus the changes that occurred simply as the result of waiting or the passage of time. Just as we say that the drug effect is the response that you obtain with a drug minus the placebo response, we can also say that the placebo effect is what we obtain from a placebo response minus what you would have gotten if no treatment of any kind were attempted. Mechanism of action The conditions that seem to be most likely to respond to placebo are those in which psychological distress plays an important role either in the exacerbation or expression of symptoms. Examples include depression, anxiety disorders, asthma, and painful conditions. Much of what we offer in the treatment situation for these conditions reduces distress. Obtaining a diagnosis and getting help from a recognized healing authority, an explanation for symptoms, and a plausible treatment are factors that alleviate distress. It is important to recall the distinction between illness and disease. Illness refers to the distress experienced by the patient, including symptoms, discouragement, and anxiety. Disease refers to objective evidence--for example, that which the physician observes in test results and imaging studies. Placebos tend to help illness--but how? Expectation has been subjected to the most study. Conditioning, endorphin release, and distress relief have also been considered, as shown in Table 2. With regard to expectation, it is clear that people experience what they expect to experience. Double-blind studies have demonstrated that when people receive a drink that they are told contains alcohol, they experience some of the effects of alcohol ingestion, including some of the physiologic effects.^10 Test subjects who are given decaffeinated coffee but are told that it is fully caffeinated exhibit improved reaction time and alertness.^10 Table 2 How placebos work Expectation Conditioning Endorphins Relief of distress Expectation has a profound effect on what people experience as a result of treatment. Until recently, the prevailing opinion held that these findings could be explained by patients telling physicians what they thought they wanted to hear or by patients imagining that they felt better. However, the results of several brain imaging studies suggest that expectation has a neuroanatomic and neurophysiologic basis.^11,12 Conditioning also has a strong impact. Responses to chemotherapy are an example. After patients have had 1 or 2 infusions of a chemotherapeutic agent that caused nausea, they often begin to feel the onset of nausea as soon as they enter the facility where the medications are administered. We are aware of patients with asthma whose airways seem to open as soon as they see their nebulizers. Conditioned responses are also seen in animals. In rats that have been the recipients of long-term amphetamine administration, extreme locomotor responses persist even when placebo is substituted for the amphetamine. In some pain experiments, placebos may increase endorphin levels, and placebo analgesia can sometimes be blocked by naloxone. An intriguing study conducted in patients with idiopathic Parkinson disease sheds more light on conditioning and expectation. These patients had been implanted with stimulating electrodes. Not surprisingly, motor performance diminished in these patients within 30 minutes of the time the stimulators were turned off. After this exercise was conducted, a sham turning-off procedure was conducted: Patients were told the stimulators were being shut off, but they actually remained on. Even so, motor velocity decreased. This is an example of the nocebo effect, in which a negative event happens as a result of a negative expectation. In another experiment, patients were told that the stimulators would be turned back on. Although the stimulators actually remained off, patients experienced better motor function. Although the expectation was not as powerful as the stimulator itself, the results reveal much about the ability of expectation to fuel treatment results.^13 The power of conditioning is demonstrated by the results of a study of 24 patients with mild to moderate hypertension who were randomized to receive placebo, atenolol, or no treatment. Before drug treatment was administered, blood pressure was similar in patients taking placebo and patients taking nothing. After patients had taken atenolol for a week, use of placebo caused a significantly greater antihypertensive response than receiving no treatment. The investigators concluded that the placebo response obtained after atenolol had been administered was not a simple residual drug effect.^14 In a study conducted among men with unipolar depression, changes in brain glucose metabolism were measured by using positron emission to-mography. Similarities and differences were assessed among patients receiving placebo and fluoxetine. A strong placebo response went hand-in-hand with regional metabolic increases involving the prefrontal, anterior cingulate, premotor, parietal, posterior insula, and posterior cingulate areas; metabolic decreases occurred in the subgenual cingulate, parahippocampus, and thalamus. These areas of metabolic change overlapped with those that were observed in patients who had responded to fluoxetine. Among patients who responded to fluoxetine, however, changes were also seen in the brainstem, striatum, anterior insula, and hippocampus. The authors concluded that changes in certain areas of the brain are probably essential to the remission of depression, whether active drug or placebo is administered. Patients who respond to fluoxetine experience brain changes that do not occur in patients who do not respond to placebo. Patients who respond to placebo experience brain changes that are similar to but less extensive than those that occur in response to fluoxetine. These changes are different from those seen in patients who do not respond to placebo.^15 Pages * * + Page: + 1 + 2 * Next Your name ______________________________ E-mail ______________________________ The content of this field is kept private and will not be shown publicly. Homepage ______________________________ Enter your comment here... * ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ [ ] Notify me when new comments are posted (*) All comments ( ) Replies to my comment Save order ( ) Oldest First (*) Newest First Refresh Comments Loading comments... Already Registered? Login Now: Login Email...________________________ ______________________________________ [X] Remember Me Sign in Register Today - It's Free! Join our growing community of healthcare professionals. Register Now PSYCHIATRIC TIMES CURRENT ISSUE Psychiatric Times Journal About Us Archives Subscribe Advertising Guide to Authors Editorial Board Reader Advisory Board Contact Us Related Articles * Referral Strategies for Patients With Co-Occurring Substance Use and Psychiatric Disorders * Behavioral Comorbidities in Rheumatoid Arthritis * Undertreatment of Tobacco Use Relative to Other Chronic Conditions * The Role of Electronic Cigarettes for Tobacco Dependence Treatment * Smoking Cessation: What’s Next? Related Videos * A Risk Stratification Approach to Alcohol Detoxification * New Insight Into the Neurobiology of Depression * Suicidal Behavior: A Separate Diagnosis * Neural Plasticity and Addiction * Grand Rounds: Neural Mechanisms of Mindfulness Training Related Podcasts * Alcoholism Research and Neuroimaging: Implications for Treatment * Update from the American Society of Addiction Medicine: New Criteria * Comorbid Schizophrenia and Substance Use—Moving Toward Improved Functional Outcomes * Self-defeating Personality Disorder: Recognition and Treatment * “Bath Salts” and “Herbal Incense”: Truly Not for Human Consumption Career Center * Featured Jobs * Resources Traditions Behavioral Health - FT Psychiatrists Adult and youth out-patient psychiatric positions Associate Chief of Staff - Salisbury VA Medical Center Outpatient - Younger Adults to Geriatrics - Pittsburgh Metro Area Medical Director / Psychiatirst Child & Adolescent Psychiatrist - Northern California More Jobs More Than Meets the Eye: Spotting Medical Mimics Why Psychiatrists Are Physicians First: Country Calm Before the Storm The Doctor/Patient Relationship Comes First, Last, and Always What Does “Affordable Care” Mean to You, Doctor? Too Much Medicine Is Bad For Our Health Take the 2013 Psychiatric Ethics Survey Now! More Jobs SearchMedica Rx Browse drugs by name: ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ Search for drugs: Enter drug name here... ____________________ (BUTTON) Search [logo.png] Register Today — It's Free! Join our growing community of healthcare professionals. Register Now © 1996 – 2013 UBM Medica, LLC, a UBM company UBM Medica Network * Cancer Network * ConsultantLive * Diagnostic Imaging * Medica Forums * Medical Resources * OBGYN.net * PediatricsConsultantLive * Physicians Practice * Psychiatric Times * Rheumatology Network * SearchMedica * The AIDS Reader About UBM Medica * Privacy Statement * Terms of Service * Advertising Information * Editorial Policy Statement * Editorial Board * Contribute to the Site * Report an Issue Find Us On... Find Us On: Share: Printer-friendly versionSend by emailPDF version Please Wait 20 seconds or click here to close § #Null Hypothesis: Latest Articles Null Hypothesis - The Journal of Unlikely Science * Home * Spoof Science * Strange but True * Straight Talking * News * Interactive * Shopping * Jobs Caption Competition Random Fact The magnetic North Pole moves by about 7 meters a day. * NewsNow * Alltop, all the top stories * WRITE FOR NULL * VISIT THE GADGET SHOP * SIGN UP FOR THE NEWSLETTER Geek of the week Nominate someone... Nominate a Geek. Email news@null- hypothesis.co.uk Click Here The Placebo Effect By Logan Wright In drug testing, a control group is necessary so that the results in the experimental group can be compared against something. Both groups are ideally identical aside from the experimental variable. The experimental group receives the drug, whereas the control group receives a placebo. The placebo is a pill or medication that appears to be the drug, but does not actually contain any active ingredients and thus has no effect on the user. Thus, both groups believe they are taking the drug and both groups expect to experience the effects. The trouble is, the placebo groups frequently do experience the drug's effects, sometimes as much as those taking the active drug! This baffling phenomenon, known as the placebo effect, is not limited to drug trials, however. Most people experience the placebo effect in various ways everyday. How our own psychological outlook can affect health is still mostly unknown, but you can be sure there are some great brains beavering over it right at this very moment. More information: Placebo effect: harnessing your mind's power to heal Back to Top Ten Things Science Hasn't Explained * Top Ten Things You Wouldn't Think Were British * Top Ten Crazy Patents * Top Five Narcoleptic Dogs Return to the top >> Share this Bookmark this article at Digg Bookmark this article at del.icio.us Bookmark this article at Slashdot Bookmark this article at StumbleUpon Email this article to a friend LATEST CONTENT * Geek Pop Returns * Doctor, Doctor... * Doctor, Doctor... * Doctor, Doctor... * Never trust a TV chef * Addicted to mobile phones * Doctor, Doctor... * History of the Vibrator * Doctor, Doctor... * Chemistry Party Video * COMMUNITY * BLOG * PODCAST * SCIENCE JOBS * LINKS * WHAT IS THE NULL? * IN THE PRESS * CONTACT * WRITE FOR US Search _________________ [searchButton.gif]-Submit Most Read Articles Database Exception (1194) Unable to perform query: SELECT * FROM article_count group by page_id ORDER BY counter DESC LIMIT 5 Table 'article_count' is marked as crashed and should be repaired § #The Brain Bank » Feed The Brain Bank » Comments Feed The Brain Bank » The Placebo Effect: A treatment of the mind? Comments Feed Ketamine: from drug of abuse to anti-depressant. Why can’t we tickle ourselves while schizophrenics can? The Brain Bank Don't be left out at sea – a science blog for everyone Skip to content * Home * About ← Ketamine: from drug of abuse to anti-depressant. Why can’t we tickle ourselves while schizophrenics can? → The Placebo Effect: A treatment of the mind? Posted on December 16, 2012 by thebrainbank When a patient known as Mr. Wright was diagnosed with terminal lymphoma (cancer of the lymphatic system), the doctors battling to prolong his life were ultimately left with no option but to try a new ‘controversial’ anti-cancer drug – Krebiozen. Although doctors and scientists remained unconvinced about the drugs effectiveness, Mr Wright was confident that it would lead to an improvement of his condition. Despite being bed-bound and in extremely poor health, just three days after his first treatment he had enough energy to get out of bed. After ten days his tumours had shrunk significantly in size, and he was well enough to go home. So what was this mysterious drug, and what caused the remission of his symptoms? Krebiozen was marketed and endorsed in the 1950s by several physicians who claimed the drug possessed anti-cancer properties. One study claimed that of 22 patients with diagnosed terminal cancer, 14 remained alive after treatment. However, other scientists failed to reproduce these results and ultimately decided that the drug was of no benefit to cancer sufferers. The National Institute of Cancer verified this decision after finding that the drug consisted of nothing more than simple amino acids and mineral oil, with no active ingredients. This drug was actually a placebo! Placebo treatments are usually given to patients in the form of sugar pills, but can also include injections and sham surgery. The key to the placebo’s success is ensuring the patient believes the treatment will improve their condition. Belief in the treatment can lead to a perceived or actual improvement of the condition. The so-called ‘placebo effect’ appears, at first glance, to make no sense whatsoever. How could a simple sugar pill alleviate serious medical symptoms? The answer may be as simple as a positive mental attitude. A patient’s mental well-being and perception of of their illness may be an important factor in influencing their medical prognosis. This is a remarkable concept, but how is it biologically possible? One suggestion is that the notion of medical intervention creates a mental cue which acts to kick-start an immune response within our bodies leading to self-healing. A similar response to seemingly unrelated external cues has in fact also been observed in Siberian Hamsters. When exposed to light levels which mimic winter days, hamsters show a depression of their immune response. If, however, they are exposed to lights mimicking summer days, the immune response increases and healing begins. I was first inspired to write this article by a programme I watched a few weeks ago on Channel 4 titled Derren Brown: Fear and Faith. This show demonstrated the power of the placebo effect through a fake clinical trial. Subjects on this trial were given a placebo drug (Rumyodin) and were told that it could inhibit feelings of fear. Over the course of few weeks, we saw each of the subjects overcome their respective fears ranging from heights and confrontation to singing in public. The drug was also effective as a cure for smoking and allergies. The strength of the placebo was enhanced by the very convincing story behind the drug’s development, including a fictitious pharmaceutical company and the use of doctors to administer the drug. The placebo’s extremely powerful effects were probably due to this attention to detail, meaning subjects were convinced that the treatment would work. So if placebos can offer such amazing results without the need for any active ingredient and all the side effects these may bring, why are they not used more regularly? The ability of a placebo to alleviate symptoms is variable both in how often they succeed and the strength of the resulting symptom alleviation. Placebos appear to be more effective when symptoms are subjective such as pain or nausea and less effective for non-subjective symptoms such as abnormal blood pressure or heart rate. A recent study in the US has suggested that genes may also play an important role in deciding whether or not an individual responds to a placebo. Preliminary results indicate that if a particular gene is present, individuals with irritable bowel syndrome are more likely to respond to placebo acupuncture. Whether this effect may be replicated for other conditions is unclear. However, these results do offer an explanation as to why some people are more susceptible to the placebo effect than others. A study from 1985 hypothesised that the placebo effect relies heavily upon a belief that the medicine will make you feel better. Indeed, one study showed that the attitude of the prescribing doctor towards both the drug and the patient significantly altered the patients prognosis. In this study patient’s responses to a placebo rose from 44% to 62% when the doctor prescribing the treatment made a conscious effort to be positive. This means that scientists are faced with a paradox when it comes to the use of placebos. Although there are clear ethical issues arising from their use, such as the controversy of introducing dishonesty into the patient-doctor relationship, ethical issues also arise from NOT using placebos. Is it unethical not to use something that could help improve a patient’s health? Despite this, the UK Parliamentary Committee for Science and Technology think that the placebo effect is unreliable and should not be used as a sole treatment on the NHS. In contrast, a study of GPs in Denmark has shown that 48% had previously prescribed placebo as treatments at least 10 times over the last year. Moreover, a study in 2004 uncovered that approximately 60% of physicians in Israel has used placebos in their practice. What is important to remember, is that placebos are not a ‘one-size-fits-all’ cure that works for everyone. The effect that placebos have can be highly variable and often unreliable. Whilst some people respond positively to treatment with placebos, others experience no change to their condition. This positive effect appears to depend not only on the type of ailment the patient is suffering with, but also their mental attitude towards the treatment. However, what is clear is that a lot more research needs to be carried out to investigate exactly how and why placebos work, and why their success is so variable. IFRAME: http://www.youtube.com/embed/wsFTgirKXHk?version=3&rel=1&fs=1&showsearc h=0&showinfo=1&iv_load_policy=1&wmode=transparent Post by: Sam Lawrence Print Friendly About thebrainbank The brain bank comprises a group of Manchester based scientists eager to enthuse and entertain with their scientific banter. To learn more about who we are see the our 'about' page. You can also find us on twitter @brainbankmanc or email us brainbankmanc@gmail.com. View all posts by thebrainbank → This entry was posted in Sam Lawrence. Bookmark the permalink. ← Ketamine: from drug of abuse to anti-depressant. Why can’t we tickle ourselves while schizophrenics can? → 3 Responses to The Placebo Effect: A treatment of the mind? 1. Jan says: December 21, 2012 at 1:37 am “What is important to remember, is that placebos are not a ‘one-size-fits-all’ cure that works for everyone. The effect that placebos have can be highly variable and often unreliable. Whilst some people respond positively to treatment with placebos, others experience no change to their condition. ” The same can be said of any individual medicine and yet we prescribe them almost indiscriminately. Reply 2. Jayarava says: December 21, 2012 at 9:35 am Great. I was particularly pleased to see the ethical question of not trying a placebo raised – at least you are taking it a bit seriously. Isn’t the placebo effect interesting? We do know that a lot of things can stimulate the immune system – though this itself has given rise to many bogus claims. It sounds like a testable hypothesis. Is there anyone testing it? Does the immune system suddenly shrink a tumour because of a positive mental attitude? Ethics questions aside, it should be testable shouldn’t it? Though how you would isolate the various factors in spontaneous remission and recovery from cancer I’m not sure, I’ve no doubt scientists will find a way if they bother to look. Not much money in it for the drug companies though eh? I think Derren Brown’s experiments detracted from the main point here. The curing of purely psychological effects, by a well known hypnotist, is much less interesting. If I was conducting a study I wouldn’t let Brown within 100 miles and a triple blind of the participants. I’ve watched a lot of his shows and he can do amazing things, but he’s a wild card. This is one in a small series of articles that have come out in the last 5-10 years which acknowledge physical alleviation of symptoms and sometimes cures for illness based on the patient’s belief that they have been given an effective treatment. It’s start, but I still see a lot of dismissal of such things as “merely placebo effect” as though it is inconsequential. Shrinking tumours are hardly inconsequential. Nor are many of the other effects that are now documented to quite a high standard of evidence gathering. I’d like to recommend a book by an anthropologist. “The End of Magic” by Ariel Glucklich. He studied tantric healers in modern day Varanasi, and came up with an interesting take on how they work to help with illness. It’s not medical science, but it might help to provide clues into the kinds mental changes that need to take place for a placebo to be effective. Reply 3. Mark Uribe says: December 23, 2012 at 7:05 am Surely the point here is that the key factor is a positive optimistic attitude, whether the “placebo” is a pill, a self help book, a religious belief, humour, a supportive partner, a pet, etc.. If I’m not mistaken, all these things have been shown to have healing benefits, so let’s not talk about pulling the wool over people’s eyes, but be open and make people aware how important one’s own attitude is and find ways to give people the support needed to keep positive. Reply Leave a Reply Cancel reply Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked * Name * ______________________________ Email * ______________________________ Website ______________________________ CAPTCHA Image Refresh Image ______ CAPTCHA Code * byWmY4BGjjm9________ * Copy This Password * ____________________ * Type Or Paste Password Here * Comment _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ _____________________________________________ You may use these HTML tags and attributes:
Post Comment [ ] Notify me of follow-up comments by email. [ ] Notify me of new posts by email. * Search for: ____________________ Search * Brain Bank Facebook IFRAME: http://www.facebook.com/plugins/likebox.php?href=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.f acebook.com%2FBrainBankBlog&width=200&height=432&colorscheme=light& show_faces=true&stream=false&show_border=true&header=false&force_wa ll=false * Recent TweetsMy Tweets * Recent Posts + Flashes of brilliance in the brain – the best Neuroscience images of 2013 + Battle of the brain’s sex differences…or not really? + The confusing science behind weight loss + Gambler’s mind: The thrill of almost winning + Should Backyard Brains bug out? * Archives + December 2013 + November 2013 + October 2013 + September 2013 + August 2013 + July 2013 + June 2013 + May 2013 + April 2013 + March 2013 + February 2013 + January 2013 + December 2012 + November 2012 + October 2012 + September 2012 + August 2012 + July 2012 + June 2012 + May 2012 + April 2012 + March 2012 + February 2012 + November 2011 + September 2011 + June 2011 + April 2011 + February 2011 * Categories + Chris Logie + Claire Scofield + Comments + Guest + Isabelle Abbey-Vital + Liz Granger + Louise Walker + Memory + Natasha Bray + News and Views + News in brief + Oliver Freeman + Sam Lawrence + Sarah Fox + Scientific method + Uncategorized * Recent Comments + Lise Eliot on Battle of the brain’s sex differences…or not really? + Grant on Flashes of brilliance in the brain – the best Neuroscience images of 2013 + The best neuroscience images of 2013 | Scott Joy on Flashes of brilliance in the brain – the best Neuroscience images of 2013 + 2013-12-27 Spike activity « Mind Hacks on Flashes of brilliance in the brain – the best Neuroscience images of 2013 + Tous mes voeux up’brain & up’spirit 2014 : flashes of brilliance in the brain – the best Neuroscience images of 2013 | Proâme on Battle of the brain’s sex differences…or not really? * Elsewhere on ScienceBlog.com The Brain Bank Proudly powered by WordPress. § FierceBiotech * FierceBiomarkers * FierceBiotechResearch * FierceBiotech * FierceBiotechIT * FierceCRO * FierceDiagnostics * FierceDrugDelivery * FierceMedicalDevices * FiercePharma * FiercePharmaManufacturing * FierceVaccines * More Fierce Network sites * Select another site * Advertise * Contact SEARCH______________ search Free Daily Biotech Industry Newsletter Get the latest on Biotech & Pharma Deals, Clinical Trials, FDA Decisions, and Key Regulatory Issues sent straight to your Inbox. Join over 100,000 biotech professionals who subscribe to FierceBiotech for FREE! Click here to see a sample. We never sell or give away your contact information. Our readers' trust comes first. [closer.png] EMAIL ADDRESS_______ SIGN ME UP FierceBiotech * NEWS * TOPICS + Biosimilars + Chutes and Ladders + Clinical Trials + Drug Safety + Emerging Markets + Europe + M&A + Outsourcing + Partnering + Personalized Medicine + Pipeline + R&D + Regulatory + Venture Capital * ANALYSIS + Download the FierceBiotech app for iPhone and iPad by Arsalan Arif + Public Citizen goes on a witch hunt at the FDA by John Carroll + John Reed sets out on a long-distance run at Roche's pRED by John Carroll + Fresh burst of biotech IPO pitches launches a busy Q4 season by John Carroll + VC funding follows tepid pace in Q3 as first-time biotech rounds shrivel by John Carroll + Industry Voices + More Commentary * FEATURES + The top 10 biotech IPOs of 2013 + 10 top drugs in biopharma's late-stage pipeline + Top 15 highest paid biopharma R&D chiefs + Top 10 generics makers by 2012 revenue + Top 10 Cardio Drugs 2012 + Top Med Tech Investments of Q3 + Top 15 Biotech VC Firms + Top 10 Pharma Companies by Employees + FierceMedicalDevices' 2013 Fierce 15 + Top 20 orphan drugs by 2018 * LIBRARY + Webinars + Whitepapers + eBooks + View All * EVENTS * JOBS + Browse Jobs + Post a Job * Marketplace * Subscribe * Twitter * Mobile Follow US Topics: Clinical Trials GlaxoSmithKline's Duchenne MD drug mirrors placebo effect in PhIII October 7, 2013 | By John Carroll Share Tools * Comment * Print * Contact Author * Reprint Over the weekend GlaxoSmithKline ($GSK) detailed the decisive failure of its ambitious Phase III study for drisapersen, a new drug designed to treat Duchenne muscular dystrophy. John Kraus, GSK's medical leader for drisapersen, unveiled more data from the failed study on the exon-skipping technology, which fell short not only on its primary endpoint for improved walking distances in a 6-minute test but also flunked out on three separate secondary endpoints, with the outcomes looking almost identical in each case with a placebo. After 48 weeks of therapy, says Kraus, the drisapersen group suffered a decline in walking distance, just as the placebo arm did. The decline was 10.3 meters in the drug arm's favor, falling short of a significant improvement and well off the more than 30-meter improvement seen in Phase II. Using the North Star Ambulatory Assessment test, the placebo arm scored 6.7 compared to a close 7.2 score for drisapersen. A velocity test gauging the time to climb four stairs was also virtually identical--a 0.12 decline in the placebo arm and a 0.14 decline in the drug group. There was a near mirror-image decline in the rate of patients' 10-meter walk/run velocity gauge. Kraus says the trial was well run and the quality outcomes they measured were good, providing a clear read out on the drug for the group they studied. Now investigators are going back over the data to see if a subset of patients benefited. And they are also running analyses on dystrophin expression to see how the therapy--in-licensed from the Dutch biotech Prosensa--influenced a key biomarker for the disease. "We need to understand if there may be a subpopulation that could have derived a benefit from drisapersen," Kraus tells FierceBiotech. "We are currently running analyses to help us understand." Subpopulation analysis is common in drug development, but it's far from ideal. A positive sign in a particular group might open the door to a new study geared for a narrow segment of the patient population, which has no meaningful therapy to rely on. But it's no guarantee of even limited success. Investors have been paying particularly close attention to the drisapersen data to see how it might reflect on eteplirsen, a DMD drug in development at Sarepta ($SRPT). Sarepta shares have soared on the positive outcome seen in a very small study of the drug, and the biotech believes it has a shot at an accelerated approval based on that study. Right now, eteplirsen is the leading therapy in the DMD field, and there's nothing in GlaxoSmithKline's latest data that changes any of that. Related Articles: Muscular dystrophy drug from GlaxoSmithKline, Prosensa fails PhIII New DMD data give Sarepta an edge in GlaxoSmithKline showdown Glaxo grabs coveted breakthrough drug status for muscular dystrophy drug Amid cheers and jeers, GlaxoSmithKline reveals Duchenne drug hit PhII goal Filed Under drisapersen, Duchenne muscular dystrophy, GlaxoSmithKline Comments View the discussion thread. Join 100,000+ Insiders SIGN UP FOR OUR NEWSLETTER EMAIL ADDRESS_______ SIGN ME UP FierceBiotech is the drug development industry's news monitor, covering biopharma deals, clinical trials, FDA decisions, and more. Subscribe to our free daily email and join the largest, most influential biotechnology news audience in the world. Sign up today! Popular Stories * Most Read * Most Shared * Is it human, animal or plant? At Bayer, they're mixing all three in new R&D recipe * U.S. domination of global biomedical R&D slips as Asian budgets swell * Merck R&D adopts its rivals' game plans in global overhaul * The 5 biggest trends in biopharma R&D in 2013 * FDA lifts a partial hold on Cell Therapeutics' leukemia drug * Flagship Ventures CEO keeps Merck aboard to inform biotech bets * [object Object] * [object Object] * Third Rock's Eleven pitches a $69M IPO to fuel its PhIII dry eye program * UPDATED: FDA spurns Sanofi's MS drug Lemtrada on fatally flawed PhIII design THE LIBRARY: WEBINAR Keys to Success for Your Integrated Summary of Safety (ISS) and Integrated Summary of Efficacy (ISE) | February 11, 1pm ET/ 10am PT | Sponsored by: Rho The integrated summary of safety (ISS) and the integrated summary of efficacy (ISE) are critical components of your new drug application (NDA) submission. This webinar will provide insights and important considerations for developing the ISS and ISE sections of your NDA. Register Today! MORE ITEMS Latest Commentary Merck R&D adopts its rivals' game plans in global overhaul The 5 biggest trends in biopharma R&D in 2013 Lundbeck stakes out a leading role for itself in late-stage Alzheimer's R&D Why did Roche kill off its Alzheimer's program for the BACE inhibitor RG7129? Meet the new publisher more EVENTS * Don't miss the First Clinical Event of 2014 - Exl's 3rd CROWN SummitJanuary 22-24 -- Philadelphia, PA * 6th Risk Evaluation and Mitigation Strategies Summit January 28-29 -- Alexandria, VA -- Westin Alexandria * Efficient Utilization of Maximo Asset Management within Life SciencesWednesday, January 8th -- 11am ET MORE EVENTS Press Releases * Johnson & Johnson Announces Quarterly Dividend for First Quarter 2014 * Lilly Sets Dates and Conference Calls for 2014 Financial Guidance and Fourth-Quarter 2013 Financial Results Announcements * Cancer Research UK and AstraZeneca sign deal to repurpose asthma drug that could fight kidney cancer * Advanced Cell Technology Appoints Eddy Anglade M.D. Executive Vice President of Clinical Development * Ocera Therapeutics Announces Election of Wendell Wierenga to Board * More Press Releases Featured Jobs * Quality Dossier Program Director - 13000004ZC Abbvie - Chicago IL United States * Senior Director, Global Knowledge & Strategic Intelligence Center Job Celgene Corporation - Berkeley Heights NJ US * Vice President, Global Medical Affairs - Breast Disease Lead Job Celgene Corporation - Summit NJ US * Sr. Director, Foreign Exchange (FX) Job Celgene Corporation - Berkeley Heights NJ US * Chief Financial Officer BioAgilytix Labs - Research Triangle Park NC USA A publication ofFierceMarkets FierceBiotech monitors biotech & pharma deals, FDA decisions, clinical trials, and more. Join more than 100,000 biotech industry leaders who subscribe to our free daily email. Click here to get your free weekly email briefing today! The FierceMarkets Network: Telecom * FierceCable * FierceDeveloper * FierceWireless:Europe * FierceWirelessTech * FierceOnlineVideo * FierceTelecom * FierceWireless Marketing & Retail * FierceCMO * FierceRetail * FierceMobileRetail * StorefrontBacktalk Healthcare * FierceEMR * FierceHealthcare * FierceHealthFinance * FierceHealthIT * FierceHealthPayer * FierceMedicalImaging * FierceMobileHealthcare * FiercePracticeManagement * Hospital Impact Government * FierceGovernment * FierceGovernmentIT * FierceHomelandSecurity * FierceMobileGovernment Energy * FierceEnergy * FierceSmartGrid Life Sciences * FierceBiomarkers * FierceBiotechResearch * FierceBiotech * FierceBiotechIT * FierceCRO * FierceDiagnostics * FierceDrugDelivery * FierceMedicalDevices * FiercePharma * FiercePharmaManufacturing * FierceVaccines Enterprise IT * FierceBigData * FierceCIO * FierceCIO:TechWatch * FierceContentManagement * FierceEnterpriseCommunications * FierceITSecurity * FierceMobileIT Finance * FierceCFO * FierceFinance * FierceFinanceIT * Home * Subscribe * Manage Newsletter Subscriptions * Advertise * Contact * Mobile Apps * RSS * Privacy * Editors * List in Marketplace (c) 2014 FierceMarkets. All rights reserved. Clicky §